Broadening the Definition of
College and Career Readiness:
A Holistic Approach
By Krista Mattern, Jeremy Burrus, Wayne Camara,
Ryan O’Connor, Mary Ann Hansen, James Gambrell,
Alex Casillas, Becky Bobek
ACT R R S 2014 (5)
ii
ACT Research Report Broadening the Definition of College and Career Readiness: A Holistic Approach
Contents
Executive Summary ...................................................................3
The Importance of Education .........................................................3
Current Definitions of CCR ...........................................................3
Multidimensional Model of CCR and Success ..........................................4
Barriers to a Holistic Approach to CCR ................................................4
Moving Toward a More Holistic Approach to CCR Across the KCareer Continuum .....5
Introduction ............................................................................6
Section 1 ..............................................................................7
The Importance of Education .........................................................7
Failing to Achieve CCR ...............................................................7
Effectively Fostering CCR ...........................................................10
Section 2 .............................................................................13
Definitions of College and Career Readiness .........................................13
Limitations of Current CCR Assessment Systems. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 17
Section 3 .............................................................................18
Multidimensional Model of CCR and Success .........................................18
Predicting Traditional Indicators of Success ...........................................19
Predicting Nontraditional Indicators of Success .......................................22
Early Indicators of CCR ..............................................................24
Section 4 .............................................................................26
Barriers to a Holistic Approach to CCR ...............................................26
Potential Solutions to Faking Issues ..................................................28
Change in Paradigm: Accountability Versus Student-Centered Development ...........29
Section 5 .............................................................................31
Moving Toward a More Holistic Approach to CCR Across the KCareer Continuum ....31
References ...........................................................................33
Report # 2014 (5)
ACT Research Report
www.act.org
© 2014 by ACT, Inc. All rights reserved. The ACT
®
college readiness assessment and ACT WorkKeys
®
are registered trademarks of ACT, Inc.
in the USA and other countries. ACT Engage
®
and ACT Explore
®
are registered trademarks of ACT, Inc. in the USA. ACT Aspire™ is a trademark
of ACT, Inc. in the USA. 2526
Executive Summary
The Importance of Education
Recent research shows that Americans value education (Newport & Busteed, 2013),
and that education yields economic benefits for individuals (Carnevale, Rose, & Cheah,
2011; OECD, 2013a) and for the country as a whole (Hanushek & Woessmann, 2008;
OECD, 2013a). The majority of new jobs created in the United States by the year 2018 are
projected to require at least a college degree (Bureau of Labor Statistics, 2013; Carnevale,
Smith, & Strohl, 2010), but the United States will produce far too few college graduates to
fill these openings.
Even if the United States did produce a sufficient number of college graduates, other
research strongly suggests that many would be unprepared for the world of work. Large
international studies (e.g., OECD, 2013b) and survey research by Casner-Lotto and
Barrington (2006) involving US employers demonstrates that a substantial percentage
of US college graduates are not prepared to adequately perform in entry-level positions.
Importantly, this general lack of readiness is evident even before individuals enter the
workforce. The high remediation rates and relatively low graduation rates for college
students suggest that many students do not graduate from high school ready for college
(US Department of Education, 2013a, 2013b). Compounding these problems is the fact
that about 20% of US students fail to even earn a high school diploma (Education Week,
2013). In summary, far too many US students lack college and career readiness (CCR).
Typically, discussion and standards surrounding CCR center on a student’s standing
on core academic skills such as mathematics and English language arts (ELA). The
thesis of this report is that the readiness issue can be remedied in part by broadening
this discussion in two ways. First, discussion around CCR can be broadened to include
students’ standing on cross-cutting capabilities and noncognitive skills, as evidence
suggests that these skills are also crucially important for education and work success
(see Section 3 of this report). Second, discussions of students’ CCR should happen at
much younger ages. Although a student’s CCR level typically is not assessed until late in
high school, research shows that college and career success can be predicted, and thus
intervened upon, much earlier (e.g., Heckman, Humphries, & Kautz, 2014).
Current Definitions of CCR
Traditional indicators of core academic skills, such as high school grade point average
(HSGPA), class rank, scores on the ACT college readiness assessment and the SAT, and
the rigor of coursework, have typically been used to define CCR, even though the various
definitions of CCR employ unique sets of indicators (ACT, 2004; Berkner & Chavez, 1997;
Greene & Winters, 2005; Wiley, Wyatt, & Camara, 2010). Differences among the various
definitions of CCR often lead to different conclusions about whether a student is ready
for college and a career as well as vastly different implications about the readiness of US
4
ACT Research Report Broadening the Definition of College and Career Readiness: A Holistic Approach
students more generally. These definitions vary on many factors, such as the indicators included, the
operational definition of college success (e.g., grades, grade point average [GPA]), and the model
used for reporting CCR.
It is important to note that these conceptualizations of CCR do not address the areas for
improvement proposed above. First, they are typically focused on students in the 11th grade,
rather than earlier grades. Second, they focus solely on selected core academic knowledge and
skills, without considering noncognitive skills or other cross-cutting cognitive capabilities. These
conceptualizations represent missed opportunities as a growing body of research has now
demonstrated that both cognitive and noncognitive skills are important to success in education and
the workforce, as discussed in this report.
Multidimensional Model of CCR and Success
Although cognitive predictors tend to be most strongly related to work success (e.g., supervisor
performance ratings), noncognitive predictors such as personality, career interests, and self-beliefs
are also reliable predictors of performance in the workplace (Barrick & Mount, 1991; Judge & Bono,
2001; Nye, Su, Rounds, & Drasgow, 2012; Schmidt & Hunter, 1998). Likewise, although cognitive
skills tend to be the best predictors of academic performance (e.g., GPA), noncognitive skills can
also reliably predict academic performance (Poropat, 2009; Richardson, Abraham, & Bond, 2012). In
fact, the personality trait conscientiousness was found in one study to predict college GPA as well as
does cognitive ability (Poropat, 2009).
When performance and other work-related outcomes are defined more broadly, noncognitive skills
take on added predictive validity. For example, on the job, several noncognitive skills are related
to important aspects of performance such as helping coworkers and being cooperative. Relatedly,
negative behaviors, such as lack of cooperation, deviant behavior, and withholding effort, also
offer predictive validity (Berry, Ones, & Sackett, 2007; Borman, Penner, Allen, & Motowidlo, 2001).
Noncognitive skills predict important outcomes as well, such as job satisfaction and the intention to
quit (Kristof-Brown, Zimmerman, & Johnson, 2005; Zimmerman, 2008). Relatedly, both cognitive
and noncognitive skills predict college retention (Radunzel & Noble, 2012; Robbins, Lauver, Le,
Davis, Langley, & Carlstrom, 2004).
Once again, it is important to note that many of these outcomes can be predicted at earlier ages
than what is the typical focus of current practices, and this is true when considering both cognitive
and noncognitive skills (Judge, Higgins, Thoresen, & Barrick, 1999). The relative lack of attention
paid to noncognitive skills as compared to cognitive skills is somewhat surprising given their clear
predictive value. The following information considers several possible barriers to using a more
holistic approach to CCR.
Barriers to a Holistic Approach to CCR
There are several possible reasons that noncognitive skills have received little attention in education
and have been excluded from the national discourse on CCR; two will be discussed here. First,
although some standardized assessments of noncognitive skills are available nationally (e.g., the ACT
Interest Inventory, the Learning and Study Strategies Inventory [LASSI], ETS SuccessNavigator),
they are used primarily for guidance and self-diagnostic, rather than decision-making, purposes. A
second important reason these measures have not been widely adopted is that they are relatively
5
ACT Research Report Broadening the Definition of College and Career Readiness: A Holistic Approach
“fakeable” and coachable. That is, the “right” answer is often easily detected by the examinee (for
example, agreeing to an item that states, “I work hard”), and thus examinees can claim to perform
behaviors that they typically do not perform. Several possible solutions to the faking problem have
been developed and are undergoing testing. As such, there is some hope that problems associated
with using measures of noncognitive skills will soon be resolved.
Moving Toward a More Holistic Approach to CCR Across the
KCareer Continuum
Moving forward, the ACT goal in establishing a holistic approach to CCR is to better assist students
and adults in identifying and navigating potential barriers at key transition points throughout the K
Career continuum. ACT is developing an expanded framework that has been empirically tested and
includes knowledge and skills related to success in education and in the workplace. This expanded
framework includes skills in at least four domains:
Core academic skills in mathematics, science, and English language arts (ELA) based on an
expanded definition of the skills and mapped to learning progressions from kindergarten through
career (KCareer).
Cross-cutting capabilities such as critical thinking and collaborative problem solving, and
information and technology skills.
Behavioral skills related to success in education and the workforce, such as dependability,
working effectively with others, adapting, and managing stress.
Education and career skills needed to successfully navigate educational and career paths,
including self-knowledge of abilities, likes and dislikes, values, etc., knowledge about majors and
occupations, and a variety of skills related to educational and career exploration, planning, and
decision making.
Each of these skills will be clearly articulated at important transition points throughout the KCareer
continuum. The ultimate goal in developing a more holistic view of CCR is to provide individuals with
feedback that is more personalized and developmentally appropriate and to offer insights in the form
of actionable information, empowering students to reach their full potential.
6
ACT Research Report Broadening the Definition of College and Career Readiness: A Holistic Approach
Introduction
A hallmark of the US education system is the opportunity afforded to students to pursue education
and career paths of their own choosing. This flexibility and autonomy, however, has drawbacks.
Students must navigate a series of complex and often disconnected environments, as well as
numerous decision points, before they attain a fulfilling career. At every transition between K–12,
postsecondary, and the workforce, obstacles can delay, and also derail, a student’s ability to achieve
success. Some students fail to graduate from high school. Many more graduate but are ill-prepared
for the next phase. To gauge whether students are on track for college and work, nationally, the
focus has been almost exclusively on core academic skills in mathematics, English language arts
(ELA), and sometimes science, as the determinants of College and Career Readiness (CCR).
However, deficiencies in other areas are just as likely to interfere with success. These other areas
include cross-cutting capabilities, which are cognitive skills that can be applied to several academic
areas. These include skills such as critical thinking, self-directed learning (study skills), and skills
needed to acquire and apply information using technology. “Noncognitive” skills are also important
for success, such as behaviors, attitudes, and education and career planning skills. Proficiency in
these areas, in combination with core academic skills, can increase a student’s chance to reach his
or her full potential.
Standards-based education reform was intended in part to address the large number of students
graduating from high school ill-prepared for college and work. The Common Core State Standards
(CCSS) are designed to help align K–12 education with the demands of college-level work in
ELA and mathematics (2012). The Next Generation Science Standards (NGSS) intend the same
for science (2013). Many other standards have preceded these efforts, beginning with the CCR
Standards first released by ACT in 1997 (ACT, 2007), Standards for Success (Conley, 2003), and
the American Diploma Project (2004). With a robust offering of standards available, the question
remains as to whether these existing standards adequately cover the knowledge and skills students
need to be ready for college and work. These standards address the core academic skills required
to transition from high school to college and work, but they do not address all of the obstacles and
challenges that students may encounter as they navigate through the education system and into
their careers. If we want to prepare students to succeed in their educational and career pursuits,
we must pay equal attention to the full spectrum of knowledge and skills proven to be essential for
success, including those that lie beyond the core academic domains.
The purpose of this paper is to demonstrate that while core academic skills are necessary, they are
not sufficient for academic and workplace success, and that a holistic approach to CCR is needed.
The paper is divided into five sections. Section 1 summarizes the current evidence on both the
importance of having a well-educated nation and the reality that the current educational system
is leaving a large percentage of students unprepared for college and work. Potential solutions to
better foster CCR are proposed. Section 2 describes previous attempts to operationalize CCR
and their limitations, notably the narrow focus on core academic skills. Section 3 includes a review
of the existing empirical evidence on the predictors of educational and workplace success, and
clearly indicates that success in education and at work is a function of a wide range of cognitive
and noncognitive skills. Section 4 addresses some of the barriers that have impeded the inclusion
of noncognitive skills in definitions, assessments, and reporting of CCR. In Section 5, ACT’s move
toward a more holistic approach to CCR is described.
7
ACT Research Report Broadening the Definition of College and Career Readiness: A Holistic Approach
Section 1
The Importance of Education
A recent poll conducted by the Gallup organization found that the overwhelming majority of
Americans believe a college education is important in today’s society (Newport & Busteed, 2013).
Only 6% of Americans said a college education was “not too important.” A full 70% said it was “very
important,” a figure that was below 40% in 1979. The clear connection between furthering one’s
education and attaining a fulfilling career may explain these results, as another Gallup poll found that
96% of Americans think education beyond high school is either “somewhat” or “very important” for
landing a good job (Gallup, 2013). These survey findings mirror actual employment trends. In 2007,
89% of all jobs in the United States required a high school diploma or higher, and 51% required
post–high school training (Carnevale, Smith, & Strohl, 2010). These requirements have steadily
increased in importance since at least 1973, when only about two-thirds of all jobs in the United
States required a high school diploma or higher.
Supporting these findings, a recent study of adult skills by the Organisation for Economic Co-
operation and Development (OECD, 2013a) found that literacy, numeracy, and problem-solving
skills are linked to positive work outcomes, such as employment and earnings. For workers in the
United States, the study revealed about a 12% increase in an individual’s wages for every standard
deviation increase in skills. In a similar vein, individuals who attain a higher level of education tend to
earn more over their lifetime (Carnevale, Rose, & Cheah, 2011). Over the course of a 40-year career,
those with just a high school diploma earn on average $1 million less (in 2009 dollars) than those
with a bachelor’s degree and nearly $2 million less (also in 2009 dollars) than those with a graduate
degree (Carnevale et al., 2011).
Education yields an economic return not only for the individual but also for the country as a whole
(Hanushek & Woessmann, 2008; OECD, 2013a). Hanushek and Woessmann (2008) looked at the
40-year growth rate of gross domestic product (GDP) in 50 countries as it related to the average
years of schooling in each country. They found that each additional year of schooling increased the
average GDP growth rate by about 0.37%. When replicating the above analyses with average test
score performance by country, these researchers found a 1% improvement in GDP growth for every
half standard deviation increase in international student achievement test scores. Note that in the
realm of economic growth, 1% is a very large number; GDP growth in the United States is currently
hovering around 2.3%. Taken together, these results clearly support the strategic value of ensuring
that each citizen receives an education that enables participation in the emerging economy. Two
problems with providing high-quality education are described as follows: (1) the current education
system is not producing enough college-educated workers to drive growth in the economy, and (2)
many graduates of the current US educational system are not adequately prepared—they do not
have the necessary skills—to succeed.
Failing to Achieve CCR
Shortages of skilled workers. The United States is currently experiencing a “skills gap” and will
continue to do so for the foreseeable future (Carnevale et al, 2010). Specifically, the US economy
will create 46.8 million new jobs by 2018, of which 63% will require a college degree or other
postsecondary credential. With only 41% of adults currently having a college degree, a shortfall of
8
ACT Research Report Broadening the Definition of College and Career Readiness: A Holistic Approach
workers with an associate’s degree or higher of about three million is projected. Similarly, from 2012
to 2022, the occupations that require a masters degree for entry are expected to experience the
greatest expansion; whereas occupations that require a high school diploma or less will have the
least job growth (Bureau of Labor Statistics, 2013).
Given the projected shortage of skilled workers, many of those jobs may need to be filled by college
graduates from other countries. For example, a recent report by the Information Technology Industry
Council found that in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM) occupations, nearly
half of all workers in the United States holding a graduate degree were born outside the country
(2012). Although the United States was once the clear world leader in terms of the percent of its
citizenry obtaining college degrees, this is no longer the case. In 2008, the United States was third
(behind Canada and Japan) in the percent of 25- to 64-year-olds obtaining at least an associate’s
degree (Carnevale & Rose, 2010). A closer inspection of the 25- to 34-year-old age group reveals
that the United States is falling even further behind the rest of the world. Just 42% of US citizens in
this age group have a college degree, ranking the United States tenth behind South Korea, Canada,
Japan, New Zealand, Norway, Ireland, Denmark, Belgium, and Australia. By comparison, South Korea,
Canada, and Japan all have at least a 55% degree attainment rate.
College graduates unprepared for the workforce. Simply increasing college completion rates
will not totally resolve the skills gap. Many US students who are graduating from college lack
the skills necessary to be successful on the job. Results of the OECD study showed that “[b]y
international standards, despite a relatively high level of educational qualifications, the basic skills of
adults in the United States are relatively weak. Unlike many other countries, there has been little sign
of improvement in recent decades. The skills of young people are little different from those of their
parents” (OECD, 2013b, p. 3).
Likewise, survey findings indicate that employers believe a large percentage of their newly hired
college-educated employees lack skills for entry-level jobs. A 2006 survey
1
of 431 employers
from across the United States found that a third of employers rated college-educated entry level
employees as “deficient” in their preparation. Another survey found that 60% of 302 employers
surveyed believe that two-year colleges and universities could use at least some improvement
in preparing students for the workforce, and even more responded that four-year colleges and
universities could use at least some improvement (Hart Research Associates, 2010). Additionally,
a 2011 survey of 2,000 US companies found that two-thirds of companies reported difficulties
finding qualified people to fill some of their open positions (Manyika et al., 2011). In 30% of these
companies, several positions had remained unfilled for at least six months. Similarly, a survey
of 1,123 executives in the manufacturing industry across all 50 states found that 55% were
experiencing a moderate shortage of qualified workers in their industry, and an additional 12% were
experiencing a serious shortage (Deloitte and the Manufacturing Institute, 2011).
A recent ACT report on the state of work readiness in the United States resonates with these
findings, concluding that, “[a] higher level of education does not always guarantee work readiness”
(ACT, 2013, p. 4). The report summarizes research showing that similar core academic skills are
required for college and work, but clearly different levels of skill in these areas may be required
across different occupations. In addition, the report concludes that noncognitive skills (e.g.,
1
The survey was conducted by the Conference Board, Corporate Voices for Working Families, the Partnership for 21st Century Skills,
and the Society for Human Resource Management.
9
ACT Research Report Broadening the Definition of College and Career Readiness: A Holistic Approach
teamwork) and cross-cutting capabilities (e.g., applied technology) are related to success at work.
One implication of these findings is that many individuals who attain a college degree still may
lack proficiency in skills required to succeed at work. In the absence of an increased effort to align
the skills and capabilities taught in school with the skills and capabilities needed to be ready for
work, a great percentage of the workforce will continue to be unprepared for work even if college
completion rates increase.
High school graduates unprepared for college. One reason so many students are graduating
from college ill-prepared for the workforce is that they are entering college with significant skill
deficiencies—a point at which it may be too late to remedy deficiencies and to get them back on
track. Specifically, national statistics indicate that even though college enrollment has been steadily
increasing since the 1950s,
2
a substantial proportion of enrollees never earn a college degree (US
Department of Education, 2013a). Of students who enrolled for the first time at a four-year degree
granting institution in 2005, only 59% had graduated six years later. The outlook is even bleaker for
institutions with open admission policies, with only 31% of students graduating within six years. A
similar story can be told for two-year degree granting institutions.
Higher education presents a broader range of choices and potential pitfalls than high school and, as
such, there are myriad reasons why a student may not make it through to degree attainment. One
particularly pervasive reason, however, is that many students are simply not academically ready to
meet the demands of a college education. This is clearly demonstrated by the fact that about 20%
of first-year undergraduate students in the 2007–2008 school year took remedial
3
courses; the rate
is even higher at two-year institutions (US Department of Education, 2013b). This high remediation
rate is a signal that the US K–12 education system is graduating many students who are not college
ready (ACT, 2004).
Corroborating the statistics on remedial course enrollment, findings from a survey of professors also
indicate that high school graduates are not adequately prepared for college. Only 26% of college
instructors said the students they teach are “Well” or “Very Well” prepared for college-level work in
the area they teach, and only 16% said that local K–12 alignment efforts were “Very Effective” (ACT,
2013b). In fact, one-third of these instructors said they were unaware of any such efforts to improve
the alignment between K–12 and college.
Research has also shown that many noncognitive variables, such as motivation, interest, self-efficacy,
and goals, are related to persistence in higher education (Allen & Robbins, 2010; Nye, Su, Rounds,
& Drasgow, 2012; Robbins et al., 2004; Schmitt, Keeney, Oswald et al., 2009). Clearly, many high
school graduates are not prepared for college. Perhaps more troubling is the fact that many students
do not even advance that far in their educational career, failing to even finish high school.
High school dropouts. Though high school graduation rates have been increasing, one out of every
five students still fails to earn a high school diploma (Education Week, 2014). Moreover, graduation
rates in the United States tend to vary by region with the Midwest and the East Coast generally
having higher graduation rates and areas such as the Southeast having lower graduation rates
2
According to the National Center for Education Statistics (NCES), enrollment in degree-granting institutions increased by 11% from
1991 to 2001, and another 32% between 2001 and 2011 (US Department of Education, 2013). This increase is happening for all age
groups, for both males and females, as well as for underrepresented minority students.
3
Remedial (sometimes called developmental) education consists of “courses in reading, writing, and mathematics for college students
lacking those skills necessary to perform college-level work at the level required by the institution” (Parsad, Lewis, & Greene, 2003,
p. 1). The goal of these courses is to prepare students to earn a degree by eliminating their academic deficiencies.
10
ACT Research Report Broadening the Definition of College and Career Readiness: A Holistic Approach
(US Department of Education, 2013a). About 15% of high schools within the United States have
annual graduation rates of less than 50% (Balfanz & Letgers, 2004). A large portion of US minority
students drop out of high schools often labeled “dropout factories,” and a high proportion of students
attending these schools live in poverty. In fact, poverty is one of the strongest predictors of high
school departure (Balfanz & Letgers, 2004). Even though current statistics indicate that high school
graduation rates are at their highest since the 1970s, roughly a million students are still dropping out
of high school each year (Education Week, 2013).
Effectively Fostering CCR
The data clearly indicate that at every point along the KCareer continuum, students are falling off
the track to being ready for college and work. This next section describes two ways to help more
students stay on track and ultimately achieve success in school and at work. First, students should
be provided with feedback that highlights their strengths along with areas for development along
a broader spectrum of skills; a sole focus on core academic skills presents an incomplete picture
of a student’s CCR. Such feedback can be diagnostic and developmental. Second, students should
be given such feedback early and often. Clearly, waiting until high school to provide such feedback
is often too late to effectively remedy most skill deficiencies because many students fall off track
much earlier in their academic career. “Catching up” is challenging, particularly for at-risk students
(Dougherty, 2014).
Broadening the definition of CCR. Standards that have been developed to help educators identify
skills that students need to improve have generally focused narrowly on mathematics and ELA, in
large part because these core academic domains are viewed as more easily attributed to schools
and specific teachers and thus more amenable to accountability. Differentiated course taking occurs
in other academic domains (e.g., foreign language, humanities, science, arts), which translates to a
much more complex accountability model for high schools. Also, cross-cutting capabilities, such as
critical thinking, collaborative problem solving, information and technology skills, and noncognitive
skills, such as behaviors, planning, goal setting, and self-knowledge, are not uniquely associated with
a specific course and cannot be easily attributed to teacher performance or school effectiveness.
Such skills may be as essential to long-term success, but they have been largely neglected because
current models of CCR appear oriented primarily to holding teachers and schools accountable,
rather than focused on student development.
Data from workforce surveys signal that a broader definition of CCR is needed if the goal is to
adequately prepare students for work. For example, in one survey of 431 US employers, skills not
traditionally considered core academic skills were more frequently rated as “very important” than
were core academic skills (Casner-Lotto & Barrington, 2006). Specifically, oral communication,
teamwork, work ethic, and critical thinking were all more frequently listed as “very important” than
were knowledge of writing in English, the English language, mathematics, and science.
Similarly, 52% of executives identified their employees’ inadequate problem-solving skills as a
serious skills deficiency; 40% identified inadequate basic employability skills, including work ethic;
whereas only 30% cited inadequate mathematics skills (Deloitte and the Manufacturing Institute,
2011). Finally, in yet another survey, an overwhelming majority of employers indicated that colleges
should place more emphasis on written and oral communication (89%), critical thinking (81%),
complex problem solving (75%), ethical decision making (75%), teamwork (71%), and innovation
and creativity (70%) (Hart Research Associates, 2010).
11
ACT Research Report Broadening the Definition of College and Career Readiness: A Holistic Approach
In general, the results from these surveys indicate that employers are less concerned with the core
academic preparation of graduates than they are with the cross-cutting capabilities and noncognitive
skills. The perceived importance of such skills has led to the current “21st Century Skills” movement,
in which several organizations have created skills frameworks that emphasize this broader array
of important skills, such as the growing importance of applied skills related to use of information
and communication technology in our society (Binkley et al., 2012; Partnership for 21st Century
Skills, 2014). In addition, recognition of the importance of developing skills in students so that they
can successfully navigate their education and career pathways has also resulted in development
of standards for that purpose (American School Counselor Association, 2004; National Career
Development Association, 2009).
Earlier feedback. Another mechanism through which CCR can be more effectively fostered is
earlier feedback. Early conceptions of CCR focused exclusively on students who had completed
high school, but there is increased recognition that students can benefit from feedback given much
earlier that will signal whether they are on track to CCR. Research shows that students who are
off-track can be identified at much earlier points in time, both in core academic domains as well
as broader skills and behaviors (Allensworth, 2005; Neild & Balfanz, 2006; Heckman et al., 2014).
Several large-scale longitudinal studies
4
of children indicate that warning signs for students at risk
for dropping out of high school can be seen in early childhood. For instance, at ages two, 12, and 14,
eventual high school dropouts were much more likely than high school graduates to have parents
who were divorced or separated (Heckman et al., 2014). Additionally, as early as one year of age,
eventual dropouts experienced less parental investment—material (e.g., access to books), cognitive
(e.g., being read to), and emotional (e.g., being disciplined)—than was experienced by eventual high
school graduates beginning at one year of age. A study of eighth- and ninth-grade students in
Philadelphia public schools found that dropping out of high school could be predicted accurately
by factors such as poor attendance rates, failing to earn a sufficient number of course credits, and
being held back in earlier grades (Neild & Balfanz, 2006).
In a 2008 research report, ACT demonstrated that CCR can be predicted based on cognitive skills
as early as eighth grade. Based on students graduating from high school in 2005 and 2006, eighth
grade ACT Explore
® 5
scores were a strong predictor of CCR in 11th and 12th grade. The report
concludes that, “…increasing eighth-grade students’ academic achievement by Grade 8 and helping
them get on target for college and career readiness would result in greater improvement in college
and career readiness than their simply taking additional standard courses or advanced/honors
courses in high school or earning higher grades in high school” (p. 36). Collectively, this research
not only demonstrates that students who may be at risk or off-track for CCR can be identified very
early in school, it also suggests that opportunities exist to intervene and ensure students develop the
range of skills necessary for CCR prior to high school.
In sum, in order to adequately prepare students to succeed in today’s workplace, definitions of CCR
will need to encompass a broader range of knowledge and skills. While it is arguably easier to focus
on core academic domains, which are more directly attributed to school and/or teacher effectiveness
and can be assessed using traditional measures, the wider range of knowledge, skills, and behaviors
4
Children of the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth 1979, the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth 1979 and 1997, and the
National Education Longitudinal Survey.
5
ACT Explore
contains four curriculum-based assessments: English, Mathematics, Reading, and Science. ACT Explore is based on
the major areas of high school and postsecondary instructional programs and measures the skills and knowledge needed for college
success.
12
ACT Research Report Broadening the Definition of College and Career Readiness: A Holistic Approach
that have been empirically linked to success both in college and the workplace must be considered
in order to more effectively foster the readiness of individual students. To gauge whether students
are on track to achieve CCR, a comprehensive framework should be created that describes in detail
the full range of skills necessary for success, offering individuals clarity about what they need to
know and be able to do at key transition points along the KCareer continuum. Before expanding on
the knowledge, skills, and behaviors necessary for success in school and at work, a brief review of
how CCR has been defined is discussed as follows in Section 2.
13
ACT Research Report Broadening the Definition of College and Career Readiness: A Holistic Approach
Section 2
Definitions of College and Career Readiness
Efforts to assess and track the CCR of US students have extended beyond learning what proportion
of students has earned high school diplomas. Various definitions of CCR have been proposed; each
is unique though all share the common goal of evaluating whether students are graduating from
high school prepared for college and work. Some approaches to defining and measuring CCR have
employed rational methods whereas others have developed empirically based benchmarks. A brief
summary of these efforts follows.
In an early example, NCES developed a college qualification index that defined five levels of
readiness based on the distribution of HSGPA, class rank, National Educational Longitudinal Study
(NELS) scores, ACT/SAT scores, and academic coursework among four-year college students
(Berkner & Chavez, 1997). The five levels—very highly qualified, highly qualified, somewhat qualified,
minimally qualified, and marginally or not qualified—corresponded to the performance of students
in four-year colleges scoring in the top 10%, 25%, 50%, and 75%, and the bottom 25% of each of
these measures, respectively.
6
Students only had to achieve one of the criteria (e.g., HSGPA ≥ 3.7)
to be classified into the relevant level (e.g., very highly qualified). Based on this classification system,
64.5% of 1992 high school graduates were at least minimally qualified; only 21.4% were deemed
very highly qualified.
Researchers with the Manhattan Institute for Public Research also examined college readiness
rates, though a different set of criteria and a different model for classification were employed
(Greene & Winters, 2005). To determine readiness, the authors examined whether a student earned
a high school diploma, completed a minimum set of course requirements—four years of English,
three years of mathematics, and two years each of natural science, social science, and foreign
language—and could read at a basic level or above on the NAEP reading assessment. Unlike the
NCES model (1997), in which classification was based on meeting at least one cut score, the
Manhattan Institute (Greene & Winters, 2005) employed a conjunctive rule in which students had
to meet all three criteria to be considered CCR. Given this distinction, it is not surprising that they
concluded that a much smaller percentage of students—34%—were ready for college. Both of the
definitions of CCR described above set benchmarks for readiness irrespective of their relationship
with actual college performance. Therefore, whether students who were deemed “college ready”
were actually successful in college—and alternatively, whether students deemed “not ready” were
actually unsuccessful—remained unexamined.
Given the lack of empirical evidence to support these initial conceptualizations of CCR, other
definitions have emerged based on predictive models and approaches to formulate CCR
benchmarks that are empirically based. Specifically, ACT released a seminal paper, Crisis at the
Core: Preparing All Students for College and Work (2004), that illustrated that the majority of high
school graduates are not ready for college-level work. This was shown by developing empirically
derived benchmarks to identify students who have a high likelihood of being successful across
6
For example, the cut scores or benchmarks for the “very highly qualified” category were HSGPA of 3.7, 96th percentile class rank,
97th percentile on NELS, and an ACT
®
college readiness assessment score of 28 or SAT score of 1250. An adjustment for the rigor
of a student’s high school coursework was also included so that students who took at least four years of English; three years each of
science, mathematics, and social studies; and two years of a foreign language were moved into the next higher level of qualification.
Students in the top two levels who did not complete rigorous coursework in high school were moved down one level.
14
ACT Research Report Broadening the Definition of College and Career Readiness: A Holistic Approach
two-year and four-year colleges based on their performance on the ACT
®
college readiness
assessment (ACT, 2004; Allen & Sconing, 2005). A 2007 study explored the use of SAT scores
to set similar benchmarks for success at four-year colleges (Korbin, 2007). Both the ACT and the
College Board (SAT) benchmarks were based on the predictive relationship between scores on
their respective college admission tests and performance in the first year of college, though the two
organizations took slightly different approaches (Camara, 2013).
ACT derived subject-specific benchmarks by calculating the ACT test score associated with a 50%
probability of earning a B or higher (which also mapped onto a 75% probability of a C or higher) in
the most typical credit-bearing course in the subject area completed by freshmen across a nationally
representative sample of two-year and four-year colleges. For example, the ACT Mathematics
Benchmark was derived based on the relationship between ACT Mathematics test scores and
grades in a college algebra course. Grades in English composition, biology, and social science
courses formed the bases of benchmarks related to scores on the ACT English, Science, and
Reading Tests.
7
The ACT focus on entry-level, credit-bearing courses was in response to the high
rates of remediation reported for students who otherwise went through the K–12 system with little
or no indication that they were academically underprepared for college (Camara, 2013).
Data support the value of the ACT College Readiness Benchmarks, as students who meet them
earn higher grades in typical first-year courses. Research also shows that meeting the benchmarks
is linked to higher first-year grade point averages (FYGPAs), second-year retention rates, and
college graduation rates (ACT, 2010; Radunzel & Noble, 2012). In 2013, only one in four ACT-tested
high school graduates (26%) met all four benchmarks (ACT, 2013).
The College Board benchmark is also based on the relationship between SAT scores and
performance in the first year of college, but it was developed by linking assessment scores to
FYGPA rather than to specific course grades. Specifically, the College Board benchmark was
estimated based on the SAT composite score
8
(SAT Math + Critical Reading + Writing) associated
with a 65% probability of earning a FYGPA of a B– or higher (Wyatt, Kobrin, Wiley, Camara, &
Proestler, 2011). Similar to the ACT results, data support the use of the College Board benchmark
in that students who meet the benchmark have more positive college outcomes—not only do they
achieve higher FYGPAs, they are also more likely to enroll in college, return for their second year,
and have higher four and six year graduation rates (College Board, 2012; Kobrin, 2007; Mattern,
Shaw, & Marini, 2013; Wyatt et al., 2011). In 2013, 43% of SAT-tested high school graduates met
this benchmark, which is considerably higher than the 26% reported as meeting all four ACT
benchmarks.
The discrepancy in the rates of CCR estimated by the ACT and College Board benchmarks is largely
attributable to differences in the methodology employed. Specifically, ACT sets a level of proficiency
in each subject area. If the definition of CCR is the percent meeting all four benchmarks, then the
model is a conjunctive one. It could be argued that students can be successful in college without
meeting each benchmark, but for the purpose of this section, we will use the standard of meeting all
four benchmarks as the definition of CCR. To meet the College Board benchmark, students simply
7
The ACT College Readiness Benchmarks were updated in 2013 based on more current data. The new analyses revealed no change
in the English and Mathematics Benchmarks of 18 and 22, respectively. The Reading Benchmark increased from 21 to 22, and the
Science Benchmark decreased from 24 to 23 (Allen, 2013).
8
In 2007, the College Board released benchmarks based on the previous version of the SAT, which did not include a writing section
benchmark, and were determined based on the composite score of SAT Math + SAT Verbal (Kobrin, 2007).
15
ACT Research Report Broadening the Definition of College and Career Readiness: A Holistic Approach
need a total SAT score of 1550. Many different combinations of scores and vastly distinct profiles
(e.g., 800 Math + 450 Critical Reading + 300 Writing as compared to 520 Math + 520 Critical
Reading + 510 Writing) can lead to the conclusion that the student is college ready.
Researchers have proposed a multidimensional model of CCR that employs HSGPA, academic
rigor, and admissions test scores (Wiley, Wyatt, & Camara, 2010; Wyatt, Wiley, Camara, & Proestler,
2011). Students had to meet the benchmarks for all three criteria to be considered CCR; and not
surprisingly, this approach concluded that only 31.9% of the 2009 SAT-tested cohort were college
ready (Wiley et al., 2010). However, it is likely that a compensatory model that balances success
among the three predictors would be equally effective in predicting CCR. Table 1 illustrates a
summary of the CCR benchmarks reviewed and how these definitions affect estimates of the
readiness of US high school graduates for college and career.
Table 1 Comparison of CCR Benchmark Definitions
Source NCES
Manhattan
Institute ACT College Board Wiley et al.
Method
Rational
Benchmark
Rational
Benchmark
Empirical
Benchmark
Empirical
Benchmark
Empirical
Benchmark
Readiness
Indicators
HSGPA, class rank,
NELS, ACT, SAT
HS diploma, core
curriculum, NAEP
ACT Test scores
SAT Composite
Score
SAT Composite
Score, HSGPA,
academic rigor
Success
Indicator
n/a n/a Course Grade FYGPA FYGPA
Level of
Success
n/a n/a B B- B-
Probability of
Success
n/a n/a 50% 65% 65%
% CCR
(Cohort)
64.5% (1992) 34% (2002) 26% (2013) 43% (2013) 32% (2009)
These various definitions of CCR illustrate that a variety of issues must be considered when
developing benchmarks, beginning with deciding whether to use rational or empirical benchmarks.
If empirical benchmarks are developed, decisions about the expected outcome, the desired
performance, and the probability of success need to be made (Camara, 2013). An additional
consideration is the type of methodology to employ for reporting CCR rates (e.g., compensatory,
conjunctive). Each of these various elements can have a profound impact on the results and the
conclusions that may be drawn, underscoring the need for thoughtful consideration of how to define
CCR to ensure meaningful generalizations.
The advent of the Common Core State Standards and the resulting formation of two multistate
assessment consortia, Partnership for Assessment of Readiness for College and Careers (PARCC)
and Smarter Balanced Assessment Consortia (SBAC), have intensified interest and debate
around CCR (Camara, 2013; SBAC, 2012). Specifically, the two consortia are each developing new
assessments that will be aligned to the Common Core State Standards. The ultimate goal is to use
16
ACT Research Report Broadening the Definition of College and Career Readiness: A Holistic Approach
the new assessments to evaluate the CCR of high school students as well as to provide diagnostic
information at earlier grades about whether students are on track for CCR. Setting benchmarks
for CCR as students transition to college and establishing benchmarks at earlier grades to indicate
whether students are on track has been a focal discussion point (Camara, 2013; Camara, &
Quenemoen, 2012; SBAC, 2012).
Today, CCR benchmarks are reported on the ACT, the SAT, and several state assessments, and
PARCC and Smarter Balanced will report benchmarks in 2015. The National Assessment of
Educational Progress (NAEP) has also been used as a metric for tracking readiness at the national
level (US Department of Education, Institute of Education Sciences, 2014). In the near future, it
is quite plausible that a half-dozen separate methodologies and definitions will be used to report
on the CCR of students across states and nationally. While all such indicators will likely attempt
to benchmark readiness in terms of postsecondary success, they also will likely employ different
empirical criteria, methodology/models, and/or judgments about the knowledge and skills required
for success. Moreover, given the content specifications of the assessments that are currently used,
or will be used to determine CCR, all indicators likely will focus exclusively on core academic skills,
and most
9
will only consider student performance in mathematics and ELA.
A related concern is whether readiness for college is the same as readiness for a career (or a job)
and whether the two should be combined into a single concept, often labeled CCR. The research
described above has focused exclusively on college students and college outcomes. A limited
number of studies have been conducted evaluating whether the level of knowledge and skills
needed for college readiness and career readiness that are measured by such assessments are
the same. The findings of such research have been generally inconclusive. Academic assessments
can certainly predict generalized cognitive skills associated with work outcomes and success, but
the specific mathematics, reading, and writing skills (e.g., statistics vs. geometric reasoning), as well
as their importance, would likely differ across careers. Subject matter experts have generally failed
to agree on such issues across careers (ACT, 2006; 2013b; ADP, 2004; Loomis, 2011; Kilpatrick,
2012).
Research indicates that a minimum level of proficiency with the core academic skills in mathematics,
reading, and writing that are needed for college readiness is important for the vast majority of jobs
10
(ACT, 2013). However, the issue becomes more nuanced when focusing on particular types of
occupations/career paths or on specific majors for which different profiles of knowledge and skills
may be required. Others have attempted to address this issue in more detail, but such a discussion is
beyond the scope of this report.
11
Still, more precise conceptual definitions of college readiness and
career readiness are needed in order to support validity arguments associated with the assessments
and accountability systems being developed.
9
The ACT College Readiness Benchmarks include math, reading, science and English. A few states have, or are attempting to,
benchmark their science assessments to college readiness (e.g., Texas).
10
Using a database of skill profiles for thousands of jobs across the country, ACT found that nearly three-quarters (73 percent) of these
jobs require at least a level 3 score on the ACT WorkKeys
®
Reading for Information, Applied Mathematics, and Locating Information
assessments.
11
See ACT (2006); ADP (2004); Camara (2013); Camara and Quenemoen (2012); Conley (2012); Kilpatrick, 2012; and Loomis, 2011.
17
ACT Research Report Broadening the Definition of College and Career Readiness: A Holistic Approach
Limitations of Current CCR Assessment Systems
CCR benchmarks have focused attention on the misalignment between the expectations and
requirements for high school and postsecondary success, but the attention is often paid too late
in the developmental process to benefit students. Preparation for, and assessment of, CCR should
begin earlier than 10th or 11th grade,
12
and it should focus on the much broader range of skills
associated with success. The narrow definitions used in current CCR systems, which focus solely
on core academic skills (i.e., ELA, mathematics, science), stand in contrast to the growing body of
research indicating that success in school and at work is a function of not only core academic skills
but other factors, such as critical thinking, motivation, work ethic, and interests. Taking a broader view
of the predictors of success is not a new idea (Campbell, 1990a; 1990b; Campbell, McCloy, Oppler,
& Sager, 1993; Camara, 2005a, 2005b; Conley, 2007).
In a similar vein, a broader view of what constitutes success and our indicators of measures of
success is also needed. The CCR benchmarks described above are based on the level of knowledge
and skills needed for a reasonable likelihood of success in first-year college courses. However,
persistence in college, cumulative grades, and progression to graduate or professional school are
also valuable indicators of college success. Many educators and policymakers argue that the true
measure of college success is degree attainment (Mattern et al., 2014). A definition of work success
remains elusive. Possible metrics include training program performance, promotion and progression
of employment, job performance, tenure, job satisfaction, and salary. Each metric results in vastly
different benchmarks of readiness, along with differing caveats regarding interpretation. Clearly,
attention to both the predictor and the outcome is needed to foster an improved understanding of
CCR.
In the next section, research findings are reviewed that address what students need to know
and should be able and willing to do, beyond core academic skills, in order to be successful in
college and at work. The argument “what you test is what you get” has been used to advocate for
performance-based assessments, more rigorous standards, or other types of reforms to educational
assessments (Herman & Linn, 2013). This same argument applies to the breadth of how to construct
and define CCR. If CCR focuses solely on core academic skills like mathematics and ELA, which
can be more directly tied to school and teacher effectiveness, this same argument would suggest
that other skills and behaviors essential for success in navigating college and careers will be
neglected and success will continue to elude many students. Broadening the definition of CCR may
help clarify how current educational systems can prepare students for college and the workforce.
12
ACT and the College Board mapped back their college readiness benchmarks to performance on their eighth- and 10th-grade
assessments (ACT, 2006; College Board, 2013; Proctor, Wyatt, & Wiley, 2010). However, giving feedback in eighth grade may still be
too late (ACT, 2008; Dougherty, 2014). Only a very small percentage of students who are off-track in eighth grade will graduate from
high school ready for college. This is particularly true for at-risk students, underscoring the need for interventions earlier in elementary
and middle school. The development of ACT Aspire™, which is a longitudinal assessment system designed to assess and track
students from third through 10th grade and provide diagnostic information and on-/off-track indicators, will help mitigate this issue.
18
ACT Research Report Broadening the Definition of College and Career Readiness: A Holistic Approach
Section 3
Multidimensional Model of CCR and Success
This section reviews the literature on the predictors of workplace and educational success, focusing
primarily on reviews and meta-analyses.
13
Reviewed predictors include core academic skills, cross-
cutting capabilities, and noncognitive skills. The terms cross-cutting capabilities and noncognitive
skills encompass a variety of characteristics, including, but not limited to, collaborative problem
solving, critical thinking, dispositional self-efficacy, goal setting, information and communication
technology skills development, personality, psychosocial factors, self-knowledge, socio-emotional
learning, and vocational interests. The findings of this literature review show that core academic
skills, cross-cutting capabilities, and cognitive ability generally predict traditional performance-based
indicators of success better than do noncognitive skills. However, when a more comprehensive
definition of success is employed, noncognitive skills become more important and sometimes even
more predictive than cognitive skills. Further, noncognitive skills provide incremental validity above
and beyond cognitive skills in predicting even the more traditional indicators of success. Table 2
provides some examples of the wide variety of predictors and outcomes that can lead to better
understanding and assessment of CCR, categorized by domain (i.e., education, work), predictor (i.e.,
cognitive, noncognitive), and outcome (i.e., traditional versus nontraditional) and highlighting the
commonality and uniqueness across school and at work.
Table 2 Indicators of College and Career Readiness and Success
Domain
Predictors Outcomes
Cognitive Noncognitive Traditional Nontraditional
College
Cognitive Ability
HSGPA
Test Scores
Absenteeism
Academic Self-Efficacy
Academic/grade goals
Achievement Needs
Behavioral Problems
Fit (interest-major)
Goal Orientation
Interests
Motivation
Personality
Self-Regulation
Social Engagement
Study Skills
Test Anxiety
FYGPA
Course Grades
Credentials/Licensure
Dropped Out
Engagement
Expelled
Graduation
Persistence
Retention
Satisfaction
Timely Degree Completion
Career
Critical Thinking
Collaboration Skills
ICT
Cognitive Ability
Degrees/Credentials
Work Samples
Fit
Integrity
Interests
Personality
Self-efficacy
Self-esteem
Values
Job Performance
Job Training Performance
CWB
Employment
Fired
Intention to Quit
Job Satisfaction
Job Tenure
OCB
Promotion/Advancement
Salary
Turnover/Quit Job
Note. ICT = information and communication technology skills; CWB = counterproductive work behavior;
OCB = organizational citizenship behavior.
13
Meta-analysis is a statistical methodology in which similar studies are quantitatively aggregated to test the pooled data for statistical
significance.
19
ACT Research Report Broadening the Definition of College and Career Readiness: A Holistic Approach
Predicting Traditional Indicators of Success
Workplace success. Cognitive predictors, in particular cognitive ability, are among the strongest, if
not the strongest, predictors of overall job performance across a variety of job situations (Colquitt,
LePine, & Noe, 2000; Hunter, 1980, 1983; Judge, Higgins, Thoresen, & Barrick, 1999; Schmidt &
Hunter, 1998; Schmidt & Hunter, 2004). Schmidt and Hunter (1998) conducted a meta-analysis
of 85 years of research in one of the most comprehensive reviews of the validity of 19 selection
methods for predicting job performance. Of all the selection methods investigated, the researchers
found that tests of cognitive ability were clearly the most predictive of job performance. This finding
has been demonstrated across several meta-analyses (Schmidt & Hunter, 2004), and for job training
performance in addition to supervisor-rated job performance (Colquitt et al., 2000). Furthermore,
cognitive ability predicts job performance across levels of job complexity and difficulty, although it is,
in fact, more predictive of performance for highly complex jobs (Hunter, 1983).
Research has shown that several noncognitive skills also predict important work outcomes. For
example, a large body of literature has developed around the hypothesis that personality predicts job
performance and attainment (Barrick & Mount, 1991; Barrick, Mount, & Judge, 2001; Judge, Higgins,
Thoresen, & Barrick, 1999; Lindqvist & Vestman, 2011; Ones, Viswesvaran, & Schmidt, 1993;
Roberts, Kuncel, Shiner, Caspi, & Goldberg, 2007; Salgado, 2003; Schmidt & Hunter, 1998; Van
Iddekinge, Roth, Raymark, & Odle-Dusseau, 2012). The most widely used taxonomy of personality
is the “Big Five,” which states that personality consists of five broad dimensions (See Ashton et
al., 2004, for a six-dimensional structure). These dimensions are: (1) extraversion—tendency to
be outgoing, assertive, and energetic, (2) agreeableness—tendency to be kind, cooperative, and
generous, (3) conscientiousness—tendency to be organized, responsible, and hard-working, (4)
emotional stability—tendency to be free from anxiety, worry, and tension, and (5) openness to
experience—tendency to be imaginative, curious, and insightful. (See John & Srivastava, 1999 for
a thorough review of the “Big Five” taxonomy). Each of these dimensions has been shown to be at
least somewhat predictive of important outcomes with some being more predictive of performance
outcomes than others.
For instance, meta-analyses have found that traits such as conscientiousness and emotional stability
consistently predict job performance (Barrick & Mount, 1991; Barrick, Mount, & Judge, 2001;
Salgado, 2003). Of these dimensions, conscientiousness is the best predictor, although measures of
personality typically do not have strong relationships with job performance. In addition, integrity tests,
which target the tendency to behave honestly, also predict job performance (Ones, Viswesvaran,
& Schmidt, 1993; Van Iddekinge, Roth, Raymark, & Odle-Dusseau, 2012). Importantly, both
conscientiousness and integrity provide incremental validity in the prediction of job performance
beyond cognitive ability (Schmidt & Hunter, 1998). Finally, a study of a nationally representative
sample of 18- and 19-year-old Swedish men found that a 25-minute interview designed to measure
characteristics such as responsibility, independence, outgoingness, persistence, emotional stability,
and initiative was a better predictor of wages and unemployment 14–21 years later than were
measures of cognitive ability (Lindqvist & Vestman, 2011).
Self-beliefs and vocational interests also help predict job performance. For example, self-esteem (an
overall evaluation of self-worth) and generalized self-efficacy (the belief in the capability for success)
were shown in one extensive meta-analysis (Judge & Bono, 2001) to predict job performance.
Vocational interests, too, have been found to predict job performance. Briefly, the predominant
20
ACT Research Report Broadening the Definition of College and Career Readiness: A Holistic Approach
model of interests states that vocational interests can be categorized into six types: Realistic (e.g.,
working with things), Investigative (e.g., science), Artistic (e.g., creative expression), Social (e.g.,
helping people), Enterprising (e.g., leadership), and Conventional (e.g., structured business roles; see
Holland, 1997, for more detail). A recent meta-analysis of 60 studies found that interests moderately
predicted job performance such that those who reported higher levels of interest performed better
on the job (Nye, Su, Rounds, & Drasgow, 2012). Furthermore, the relationship was stronger when
interests were congruent with jobs (e.g., a person with investigative interests worked in a job
requiring investigation). As with personality, meta-analysis has demonstrated that interests provide
a small amount of incremental validity to the prediction of job performance beyond cognitive ability
(Schmidt & Hunter, 1998).
Academic success. Similar to career success, cognitive skills have typically been shown to be
very strong predictors of traditional definitions of academic success (Poropat, 2009; Richardson,
Abraham, & Bond, 2012). In a comprehensive meta-analysis of predictors of academic performance
at all levels of schooling, Poropat (2009) found that core academic skills (e.g., ACT/SAT scores)
were strong predictors of course grades at each educational level (i.e., elementary, secondary, and
postsecondary; see also Richardson et al., [2012] for a recent meta-analysis of college performance
only). National validity studies have reached the same conclusion with results indicating that ACT
and SAT scores predict college GPA throughout the college career (Mattern & Patterson, 2011a;
Patterson & Mattern, 2011; Radunzel & Noble, 2012), and performance on the GRE and the
Miller Analogies Test predict academic performance in graduate and professional school (Kuncel,
Hezlett, & Ones, 2001; Kuncel, Hezlett, & Ones, 2004; Linn & Hastings, 1984). Additionally, meta-
analysis has demonstrated that study skills provide incremental validity above college admissions
tests for the prediction of first-year college GPA (Credé & Kuncel, 2008). Others have described
similar validity evidence that supports the use of admissions tests for undergraduate, graduate,
and professional programs across a variety of criteria, including grades, retention, and graduation
(Camara, Packman, & Wiley, 2012).
Of course, stories abound about students who may have failed academically despite having
strong cognitive skills, while other students may have succeeded despite deficits in cognitive
skills. Consistent with these stories is a growing body of research evidence suggesting the crucial
importance of taking noncognitive skills into account to better understand and predict academic
success. In terms of personality, Poropat (2009) found that each of the “Big Five” dimensions of
personality predicted academic success in elementary school. Although most dimensions became
less predictive of success in later school years, conscientiousness remained a strong predictor. In
fact, Poropat found that conscientiousness predicted academic performance in college just as well
as did cognitive skills. In addition, Richardson et al., (2012) found that conscientiousness provided
incremental prediction of college GPA above and beyond SAT/ACT scores and high school GPA
(see also O’Connor & Paunonen, 2007; Trapmann et al., 2007, for meta-analyses of the relationship
between personality and performance in college).
Strong meta-analytic evidence exists that motivational factors such as vocational interests,
goals, and achievement needs are related to academic performance (Nye et al., 2012; Robbins,
Lauver, Le, Davis, Langley, & Carlstrom, 2004; Roseth, Johnson, & Johnson, 2008). For instance,
vocational interests are moderately correlated with academic performance in college, and these
correlations are stronger when interests are congruent with the student’s major (Nye et al., 2012).
Finally, extensive meta-analyses conducted by Robbins et al., (2004) and Richardson et al., (2012)
21
ACT Research Report Broadening the Definition of College and Career Readiness: A Holistic Approach
found that academic goals (e.g., commitment to goals), grade goals, test anxiety, and achievement
motivation each predicted college GPA incrementally over other predictors such as high school GPA
and ACT/SAT scores.
Research on ACT Engage
®
(formerly known as the Student Readiness Inventory), which assesses
motivation, social engagement, and self-regulation, clearly illustrates the importance of noncognitive
skills above and beyond academic skills in the prediction of future academic success (Casillas,
Robbins, Allen, Kuo, Hanson, & Schmeiser, 2012; Robbins et al., 2004). Figure 1 highlights the fact
that high ACT Engage scores (measured in seventh or eighth grade) may even compensate for
lower ACT Explore
®
scores for some students. Specifically, students with high ACT Engage scores
but ACT Explore scores in the middle two quartiles had higher high school GPAs (HSGPAs) (mean
= 3.01) than students with low ACT Engage scores and ACT Explore scores in the top quartile
(mean = 2.57). A similar pattern of results emerged when alternative indicators of success, such as
college enrollment, college GPA, and retention, were examined.
Another important consideration in predicting academic success is how students view themselves:
Do they feel they are capable of achieving success in school? Results from two meta-analyses
found that academic self-efficacy, or the belief that one can succeed in school, predicts academic
performance above and beyond traditional predictors of college success (Brown et al., 2008;
Robbins et al., 2004), indicating that variables related to self-confidence are clearly important for
academic success.
1.47
1.83
2.57
1.80
2.45
3.27
2.26
3.01
3.62
0.00
0.50
1.00
1.50
2.00
2.50
3.00
3.50
4.00
Bottom 25% Middle 50% Top 25%
Average High School GPA
ACT Explore Composite Score
Low ACT Engage Moderate ACT Engage High ACT Engage
Figure 1. Average HSGPA by ACT Explore and ACT Engage Scores
22
ACT Research Report Broadening the Definition of College and Career Readiness: A Holistic Approach
Predicting Nontraditional Indicators of Success
Workplace success. Industrial-organizational psychology has long recognized that performance
on the job requires more than just completing tasks in a timely manner with sufficient quality.
Campbell (1990a) advanced an eight-factor model of performance that includes task-specific
behaviors, non-task-specific behaviors, oral communication, effort, personal discipline, teamwork,
and supervisory or leadership and managerial skills. Similarly, contextual performance goes
beyond just task performance and includes “…behaviors that contribute to the psychological,
social, and organizational context of work” and has been shown to be an important component of
job performance (Borman, & Motowidlo, 1993). In essence, behaviors associated with contextual
performance make employees’ jobs easier or more difficult by influencing the environment within
which they must work. A great deal of research is available that focuses on organizational citizenship
behavior (OCB), and counterproductive work behavior (CWB).
Valued by organizations—especially as it relates to customer service, teamwork, and managing
others—OCB includes positive behaviors such as helping coworkers, being cooperative, and putting
forth extra effort to get the job done. Although little research has been conducted investigating the
relationship of cognitive skills to OCB, several meta-analyses have demonstrated that noncognitive
skills are consistently related to OCB (Borman, Penner, Allen, & Motowidlo, 2001; Chiaburu, Oh,
Berry, Li, & Gardner, 2011; Hurtz, & Donovan, 2000; Judge, Rodell, Klinger, Simon, & Crawford, 2013;
Nye et al., 2012; Organ, & Ryan, 1995). The meta-analysis by Nye et al., (2012) found that interests
were related to OCB, and that these relationships were substantially stronger when interests were
congruent with jobs. Furthermore, with the exception of openness to experience, each of the “Big
Five” personality dimensions reliably predicted OCB (Judge et al., 2013). These four dimensions
predicted OCB better than they predicted performance on job tasks, providing evidence that these
noncognitive skills may take on additional importance when the definition of success is expanded.
By contrast, CWBs include behaviors such as a lack of cooperation, deviant behavior, and
withholding effort (Motowidlo, 2003). Organizations are exceedingly interested in minimizing CWBs
as they have been linked to costly organizational outcomes such as theft, absenteeism, and safety
violations. Once again, several meta-analyses have demonstrated that noncognitive skills predict
CWB (Berry, Ones, & Sackett, 2007; Judge, Klinger, Simon, & Yang, 2008; Nye et al., 2012; Ones
et al., 1993; Salgado, 2002; Van Iddekinge et al., 2012). Nye et al., (2012) found that interests were
related to CWB, although in this case, the relationships were not larger when taking into account
interest-job congruence. In addition, emotional stability, agreeableness, and conscientiousness
reliably predicted both deviant behaviors toward individuals (e.g., violence, gossip, theft) and toward
the organization (e.g., lack of effort, damaging company property, sharing confidential information)
(Berry et al., 2007). Furthermore, in Project A of Campbell’s research (1990b), which included
approximately 800,000 soldiers enlisted in the US Army, personality predicted personal disciplinary
problems and adherence to regulations better than did cognitive ability (McHenry et al., 1990).
Integrity tests have also demonstrated moderate relationships with CWBs (Ones et al., 1993; Van
Iddekinge et al., 2012). Similar to the findings described above regarding personality and OCBs,
integrity tests are better predictors of CWBs than they are of performance on job tasks, again
suggesting the value in considering these noncognitive skills when considering broader definitions
of success (Ones et al., 1993).
23
ACT Research Report Broadening the Definition of College and Career Readiness: A Holistic Approach
Job satisfaction has also been studied in depth as a key work outcome of concern. As with OCB
and CWB, research shows that job satisfaction is consistently predicted by noncognitive skills,
while the relationship of job satisfaction to cognitive ability is rarely examined. Meta-analyses
have found that job satisfaction is predicted by: each of the “Big Five” personality traits except
openness to experience (Judge & Bono, 2001; Judge, Heller, & Mount, 2002), the extent to which
a person’s values and interests fit the job and the organization (Kristof-Brown, Zimmerman, &
Johnson, 2005), self-esteem (Judge & Bono, 2001), and generalized self-efficacy (Judge & Bono,
2001). Furthermore, a longitudinal study demonstrated that occupational self-esteem predicted
career satisfaction after seven years of experience, whereas college GPA was unrelated to career
satisfaction (Abele & Spurk, 2009).
One possible outcome of job dissatisfaction is leaving the job (via the intention to quit) (Zimmerman,
2008), and noncognitive skills have also been shown to predict job tenure and persistence-
related outcomes. Meta-analyses have shown that noncognitive skills predict some of the likely
antecedents to the decision to leave the job (Alarcon, Eschleman, & Bowling, 2009; Van Iddekinge,
Putka, & Campbell, 2011; Zimmerman, 2008). For instance, emotional exhaustion on the job is
predicted by self-esteem, generalized self-efficacy, locus of control, optimism, emotional stability,
extraversion, conscientiousness, and agreeableness (Alacron, et al., 2009). Additionally, intention
to quit is predicted by all of the “Big Five” personality dimensions except openness to experience
(Zimmerman, 2008), interests (Van Iddekinge et al., 2011), and person-job and person-organization
fit (Kristof-Brown et al., 2005). Personality predicts the intention to quit incrementally over cognitive
ability, and interests further predict incrementally above and beyond both cognitive ability and
personality (Van Iddekinge et al., 2011). Finally, personality, vocational interests, and interest-job
congruence predict persistence on the job and turnover (Nye et al., 2012; Salgado, 2002).
The value of noncognitive skills is evident when the definition of success is expanded beyond
traditional indicators of job performance. That said, greater focus and rigor has been applied to
research on outcomes and criterion measures of performance in work settings (see Motowidlo,
2003) than to educational research. Research examining an expanded outcome space for academic
success, summarized in the Section 4, is much more limited.
Academic success. Of the less-traditional outcomes that have been examined in academics, the
majority of research has focused on persistence and graduation. Perhaps not surprisingly, cognitive
skills are of crucial importance for each of these outcomes. Students with higher HSGPAs and
ACT/SAT scores have a better chance of persisting and graduating from college than students with
lower academic credentials (Bowen, Chingos, & McPherson, 2009; Mattern & Patterson, 2011b;
Radunzel & Noble, 2012; Robbins et al., 2004). This is true for students at both two-year and four-
year colleges (Radunzel & Noble, 2012). Furthermore, and probably not surprisingly, college GPA is
the single best predictor of college persistence (Pascarella & Terenzini, 2005). This is reasonable
because grades, as a measure of student achievement, incorporate both cognitive (i.e., learning) and
noncognitive (i.e., motivation, dependability) components when used as a predictor of later success.
Finally, other academic skills (e.g., study skills) predict persistence incrementally over predictors such
as HSGPA and ACT/SAT scores (Robbins et al., 2004). While persistence and graduation are clearly
impacted by cognitive skills, they are not the only important predictors that should be considered.
The Robbins et al., (2004) meta-analysis provides strong evidence to support the claim that
noncognitive skills are important factors in college persistence. Specifically, results reveal that
24
ACT Research Report Broadening the Definition of College and Career Readiness: A Holistic Approach
achievement motivation, academic goals, and academic self-efficacy are all correlated with
persistence, and academic goals and academic self-efficacy predict incrementally above and beyond
cognitive predictors. Interests also predict persistence (Nye et al., 2012); and, once again, these
relationships are stronger when interests are congruent with college major. Corroborating these
findings, Allen and Robbins (2010) found that interest-major congruence as well as motivation—
as measured by the 10-item academic discipline scale of ACT Engage—predict timely degree
attainment (graduating in four years from four-year colleges, and in two years from two-year
colleges) above and beyond prior test scores and college GPA. A likely antecedent to college
persistence is absenteeism. A study of students from 10 universities found that behaviors such
as leadership and ethics predicted student absenteeism, as did a situational judgment test (SJT)
designed to measure several behavioral skills (Schmitt et al., 2009).
Early Indicators of CCR
The research previously discussed suggests that rather than focusing on core academic skills, a
more holistic approach is essential if the desire is to help students improve their prospects for the
future. The bulk of the literature already reviewed in this report focused on success at work and in
college, largely because the most extensive research on noncognitive skills has been conducted
at those levels. However, providing people with guidance about their strengths and weaknesses
just prior to their entering college or the workforce may be too late. Fortunately, evidence from
longitudinal studies indicates that individuals who may be at risk can be identified much earlier in life.
In the realm of cognitive skills, longitudinal studies of children as young as age six have found that
future high school dropouts score substantially lower on achievement tests and significantly higher
on measures of behavioral problems than do high school graduates (Heckman et al., 2014). The
likelihood of dropping out of high school can also be predicted highly accurately in the eighth grade
using mathematics and English grades (Neild & Balfanz, 2006). ACT (2008) found that scores
on the eighth grade ACT Explore test were strong predictors of readiness for college in 11th and
12th grade. Furthermore, a 10-year longitudinal study found that cognitive ability scores for 10th
grade students predicted academic attainment and earnings 10 years later (Lleras, 2008). Finally,
childhood cognitive ability predicted wages, occupation status, and job satisfaction 60 years later
(Judge, Higgins, Thoresen, & Barrick, 1999).
Several longitudinal studies attest to the usefulness of noncognitive skills for predicting long-
term success in school and at work (see Roberts, Kuncel, Shiner, Caspi, & Goldberg, 2007 for a
comprehensive review of personality as a predictor). Moon (2012) found that, at age six, those who
would eventually drop out of high school scored significantly higher on measures of behavioral
problems than high school graduates. Research also indicates the importance of conscientiousness
in children. Attention skills (considered to be related to future conscientiousness) at the age of
school entry predicted future achievement test scores incrementally over school-entry academic
skills (Duncan et al., 2007 as cited in Almlund, Duckworth, Heckman, & Kautz, 2011); childhood
conscientiousness predicted job satisfaction, wages, and occupational status above and beyond
cognitive ability 60 years later (Judge et al., 1999); and teacher ratings of 10th grade students’ work
ethic predicted educational attainment and earnings incrementally over cognitive ability 10 years
later (Lleras, 2008). Furthermore, a meta-analytic summary of longitudinal studies of elementary
school students revealed that self-beliefs such as self-efficacy, self-esteem, and self-concept
(general beliefs about the self that influence actions) predicted later academic achievement, such
25
ACT Research Report Broadening the Definition of College and Career Readiness: A Holistic Approach
as test scores and grades, even after controlling for prior achievement (Valentine, DuBois, & Cooper,
2004). Finally, the vocational preferences of gifted 13-year-olds predicted their college major 10
years later incrementally over cognitive ability (Achter, Lubinski, Benbow, & Eftekhari-Sanjani, 1999).
The fact that future success can be accurately predicted at such an early age offers hope that
at-risk children can be identified and provided with interventions. Focusing solely on core academic
skills may result in failing to identify and help at-risk students and adults who have strong academic
skills but need help developing the other skills that are essential to CCR.
26
ACT Research Report Broadening the Definition of College and Career Readiness: A Holistic Approach
Section 4
Barriers to a Holistic Approach to CCR
Scholastic aptitude admits of other things than intelligence; to succeed
in his studies, one must have qualities which depend on attention, will, and
character; for example, a certain docility, a regularity of habits, and especially
continuity of effort. A child, even if intelligent, will learn little in class if
he never listens, if he spends his time playing tricks, in giggling, in playing
truant. (Binet, 1916, p. 254)
Section 3 reviewed compelling evidence that to be ready to succeed in college and at work requires
more than just having core academic skills. Yet, current definitions of CCR tend to focus solely on
core academic achievement. One may wonder if definitions of CCR lack indicators of these other
skills because their value has only just recently been recognized. Interestingly, the creator of the
first standardized intelligence test, Alfred Binet, acknowledged nearly 100 years ago the limitation
of focusing solely on intelligence, as indicated in the quote above, which suggests that this is not
newfound knowledge.
Some have argued that there are historical reasons that may have deterred the teaching of
these other student characteristics (Heckman et al., 2014). The secularization of the K–12 public
educational system following the US Supreme Court decision in 1948 to mandate the separation of
church and state may have led to the belief that the development of character was no longer under
the purview of the public educational system. The current education reform movement, including
the No Child Left Behind legislation and the more recent adoption of the Common Core State
Standards, has further restricted the focus of what is taught in the classroom (Hamilton, Stecher,
Marsh, McCombs, & Robyn, 2007). The pendulum may now be swinging in the opposite direction
through developing and implementing many school-based programs with the very goal of integrating
development of academics, behavior, and character, as research has clearly shown that they are
interrelated and jointly influence subsequent learning and performance (Flay & Allred, 2010). More
recently, a special section of Education Week (June 5, 2014) underscored this notion for teachers,
researchers, and policymakers alike, emphasizing the need to focus not only on academics but also
on engagement and motivation in order to truly foster student learning.
Clearly, educators recognize the value of noncognitive skills. This is evident in the longtime practice
of giving conduct grades and detentions/suspensions for poor behavior. Even at the postsecondary
level, colleges and universities often include interviews, essays, and letters of references as part of
the admission application process to glean some of these additional skills (Clinedinst & Hawkins,
2009). Even though educators recognize the importance of these additional skills, they are rarely
formally taught; moreover, they are excluded from definitions of CCR.
One issue surrounding the inclusion of these skills in definitions of CCR concerns available
assessments. Measures of noncognitive skills are often locally developed and subjectively scored
with limited, if any, research evidence to support the reliability and validity of their scores’ uses
(Highhouse, 2008; Mattern, Kobrin, Patterson, Shaw, & Camara, 2009). Standardized measures
of these traits have demonstrated efficacy and validity in research studies and in their limited
27
ACT Research Report Broadening the Definition of College and Career Readiness: A Holistic Approach
operational use, but they have not been widely adopted as standardized assessments in education.
14
A discussion of a few examples of emerging approaches to more systematic assessment of
noncognitive skills follows.
ACT Engage was developed in 2005 to measure a student’s level of motivation, social engagement,
and self-regulation (ACT, 2007; Casillas et al, 2012). ACT Engage is available to students in middle
school and high school, and to students matriculating to college, but it is taken only by a small
percentage of students nationally at each level.
First developed in 1987, LASSI is popular in higher education as a diagnostic tool that identifies
study skills deficiencies among enrolled students (Weinstein, Schulte, & Palmer, 1987). In response
to the need to provide earlier feedback and assist students with the high school to college transition,
a high school version was also developed.
ETS has introduced the Personal Potential Index (Kyllonen, 2008), which is a standardized
recommendation letter designed to improve the graduate school admission decision process by
assessing knowledge and creativity, resilience, communication skills, planning and organization,
teamwork, and ethics and integrity. More recently, ETS developed SuccessNavigator (Markle, Olivera-
Aguilar, Jackson, Noeth, & Robbins, 2013), which is designed to identify at-risk college students
with the goal of improving retention and graduation rates. Neither the Personal Potential Index nor
SuccessNavigator has been widely adopted.
Despite years of research, other efforts to measure student qualities, such as the perseverance,
study skills, and self-monitoring skills of undergraduates and law school students, have not resulted
in any operational assessments (Andberg, 2007; Schmitt et al., 2009; Shultz & Zedeck, 2011).
Despite the limited use of these standardized measures, it is indisputable that skills that extend
beyond core academic skills are viewed as important by admissions officers. A survey of college
admissions officers found that after grades and test scores, the next most important factors in the
college admission process are these personal characteristics gleaned from essays, a student’s
demonstrated interest, and teacher/counselor recommendations (Clinedinst, Hurley, & Hawkins,
2011).
Adoption of noncognitive assessments in the workplace has been more widespread than in schools.
Based on a survey of 1,627 human resource managers in 2001, 8% of employers use interest
inventories, 14% of employers use managerial assessments, and 13% of employers use personality
assessments (American Management Association, 2001). Even so, assessment of cognitive skills
and abilities is still more prevalent with 41% of employers testing basic literacy and/or math skills
and 20% of employers using cognitive ability assessments. A more recent poll indicated that 71%
of human resource professionals believe personality assessments are useful for predicting work
outcomes, however, only 18% use personality assessments when hiring or promoting employees
(Society for Human Resource Management, 2012). These findings may help explain the survey
results described in Section 1, which found that employers are more concerned with the levels of
noncognitive skills their newly hired employees demonstrate as compared to their core academic
preparation. Specifically, if employers are using measures of cognitive skills to make hiring decisions,
14
ACT does administer its Interest Inventory for free when examinees register for the ACT and therefore data on nearly two million
students is collected annually. The large percentage of ACT-tested students who complete the Interest Inventory affords the
opportunity to provide more integrated, personalized feedback by taking into account the students’ achievements, interests, and
educational and career plans.
28
ACT Research Report Broadening the Definition of College and Career Readiness: A Holistic Approach
then it is likely that applicants who are offered a job will have these skills. Likewise, selection
batteries that do not include noncognitive measures allow for the possibility of hiring applicants with
poor noncognitive skills.
Issues to consider in high-stakes decisions. The “fakeability” and coachability of many of the
available noncognitive measures (e.g., personality) are often primary barriers to implementation,
especially if assessments are to be scaled up on a national level (Griffith & Converse, 2011; Lievens,
Buyse, Sackett, & Connelly, 2012). Specifically, concern with using some of these alternative
measures for high-stakes decision making—such as admitting students to college, hiring a job
applicant, or evaluating a teacher’s effectiveness—is that it is possible for responders to fake, or
intentionally distort, their responses in order to present themselves more favorably. Consider, for
example, a typical conscientiousness item: “I work hard,” with response options ranging from strongly
agree to strongly disagree. An individual who wants to be accepted to college or hired can easily
infer that the “right” answer is “strongly agree.” The question is not whether these types of measures
can be faked but whether individuals do fake and, if so, what the consequences are for faking.
Based on multiple pieces of evidence, Griffith and Converse (2011) estimated that roughly 30% of
applicants in a selection setting fake. Based on this estimate and a selection ratio of 30%, Sackett
(2011) estimated that more than half of applicants selected based on their faked scores would have
not been selected based on their honest responses. Clearly, faking can have a significant impact on
decisions made at the individual level.
Though not driven by the self-promotion intent, as is the case with faking, lack of self-awareness
can have equally deleterious effects on the usefulness of these alternative measures. A large body
of research drives home the point that individuals are often poor judges of their own skill levels
(Dunning, Heath, & Suls, 2004). Specifically, on average, individuals tend to believe they are above-
average as compared to their peers (Mattern, Burrus, & Shaw, 2010). This over-estimation is most
severe among those who are below-average (Austin & Gregory, 2007; Ehrlinger, Johnson, Banner,
Dunning, & Kruger, 2008, Kruger & Dunning, 1999; Mattern et al., 2010). Therefore, self-report
measures of these additional skills are susceptible to invalid responses, due to both intentional and
non-intentional processes, and this may be exacerbated in high-stakes decision contexts (Burrus,
Naemi, & Kyllonen, 2011).
Potential Solutions to Faking Issues
Innovative item types. Given the concern with invalid responding for many measures of
noncognitive skills, which often employ a self-report, Likert-type format, much research has been
devoted to developing item types and measures that are less prone to faking, such as forced-choice
item types, situational judgment tests, and other-report measures (ACT, 2007; Kyllonen, 2008;
McDaniel, Morgesen, Finnegan, Campion, & Braverman, 2001; Stark, Chernyshenko, & Drasgow,
2005). For example, both ACT and ETS have created measures that use ratings from teachers
to assess students’ noncognitive skills, such as academic discipline and perseverance, mitigating
concerns around socially desirable responses (ACT, 2007; Kyllonen, 2008). Use of these measures
may be limited for some intended purposes (e.g., using teacher raters for teacher evaluation
applications). Other-report formats may also be useful when there is concern about the students’
level of self-awareness; the ratings provided by others can additionally serve a formative purpose by
promoting self-awareness via others’ feedback.
29
ACT Research Report Broadening the Definition of College and Career Readiness: A Holistic Approach
Other item types have been created to reduce socially desirable responses. One promising example
is the forced-choice item format (Christiansen, Burns, & Montgomery, 2005; Heggestad, Morrison,
Reeve, & McCloy, 2006; Stark et al., 2005). Rather than asking individuals to rate themselves on
the traditional single dimension (e.g., “I work hard”), this format requires respondents to choose
which statement is more descriptive of themselves among an item pair that has been matched in
terms of social desirability and the level of the trait (e.g., “I work hard” versus “I work well in teams”).
This format is more difficult for respondents to fake successfully. Findings from the military, which
has successfully used forced-choice personality measures operationally for selection purposes,
indicate that the personality scores are predictive of important outcomes such as attrition (Stark,
Chernyshenko, & Drasgow, 2010). These findings show promise for applying these measures to
high-stakes applications in education and work.
Additional item formats that may be less prone to faking/invalid responding include situational
judgment tests and biographical data (biodata), both of which have been extensively researched
and validated in both the workplace and educational contexts (Lievens, Buyse, & Sackett, 2005;
McDaniel et al., 2001; Mumford & Owens, 1987; Oswald, Schmitt, Kim, Ramsay, & Gillespie, 2004).
Situational judgment items include a description of a scenario, in written, audio, or video format,
followed by a list of response options from which respondents are asked to make a selection (e.g.,
select the best option or the option that typifies how they would most likely handle the situation).
Biodata items ask respondents about past behaviors and experiences in a standardized manner.
Specifically, a typical biodata item asks about the frequency with which the respondent performed
a certain behavior (e.g., worked late) over a certain period of time (e.g., last month). While generally
less fake-resistant than the aforementioned item types, research has found that using elaboration
or asking respondents to provide details to verify their response, can minimize distortion on biodata
items (Schmitt & Kuncel, 2002). Other less-commonly employed item formats that hold promise
based on research findings include self-estimates of how others would respond to an item (Prelec,
2004), conditional reasoning tests (James, 1998), and vignettes to anchor the self-assessment
(King, Murray, Salomon, & Tandon, 2004). Gaming may also be a means for assessing certain
cognitive skills and behaviors and might better engage students in assessment activities. The
growing body of research on item types suggests that developing reliable, valid, and fake-resistant
assessments of these additional student characteristics for high-stakes situations may be possible.
Change in Paradigm: Accountability Versus Student-
Centered Development
Regardless of the extent to which the response distortion issues are resolved, changing how
assessments are used will be important in order to fully realize the value of assessing noncognitive
skills that promote student learning. That is, if the purpose of assessment is focused on holistic,
developmentally relevant insights for individuals and educators rather than on school and educator
accountability, then formative information concerning the individual’s development would benefit
greatly. Diagnostic information detailing individuals’ strengths and areas for development across not
only academic skills but also cross-cutting capabilities and an array of noncognitive skills to provide
personalized, integrated insights holds great promise for effectively using such assessments. Skills
across these various dimensions are essential for student learning and success along the KCareer
continuum. Given their joint influence on learning and the compound effect over time, if skills in any
of these areas need to be developed but are not identified early and acted upon, the discrepancy
30
ACT Research Report Broadening the Definition of College and Career Readiness: A Holistic Approach
between where one should be and where one is may be too great to effectively remedy and get the
individual back on track to CCR (Cunha & Heckman, 2007; Dougherty, 2014; Masten & Cicchetti,
2010). A focus on helping students realize their true potential clearly points to the importance of
adopting and implementing a broader definition of CCR beyond core academic skills.
31
ACT Research Report Broadening the Definition of College and Career Readiness: A Holistic Approach
Section 5
Moving Toward a More Holistic Approach to CCR Across the
KCareer Continuum
Research clearly shows that college and career success is multidimensional and has been assessed
using a wide variety of indicators. Grades, persistence, engagement, and graduation are all viewed
as important outcomes for college success (Camara & Quenemoen, 2012; Schmitt et al., 2009)
and have shown that universities and colleges value many qualities beyond academic success
in developing and educating their students, including leadership and perseverance. Similarly,
performance on the job and success in the workplace are viewed as multidimensional in nature.
Research demonstrates that job performance can be defined by multiple criteria and employers
often differ in the value attributed to specific outcomes and behaviors (Campbell et al., 1993).
Organizations not only value task performance as an important component of workplace success but
also other outcomes, such as OCBs and CWBs (Motowidlo, 2003). If all of these various dimensions
are important components of success, it follows that readiness and preparation should similarly be
focused on a broad and diverse set of individual differences, including core academic skills, cross-
cutting capabilities, and noncognitive skills.
To foster academic and work success, it is necessary to have a holistic definition of CCR that
focuses on the development of the knowledge and skills that can improve an individual’s chance of
success across a range of outcomes in postsecondary and work settings. A more comprehensive
approach to assessment, designed to proactively identify and accurately measure the multiple
dimensions of readiness and assist students in their development throughout the kindergarten
through career continuum, can support the implementation of such a model.
Throughout this report, evidence is presented that indicates: (1) educators, policymakers, and
employers have embraced a broad repertoire of skills as critical for success in education and work;
and (2) including more elements from that broad repertoire of skills can indeed provide incremental
validity beyond core academic skills in predicting CCR. Yet, teaching, learning, and assessing
students in kindergarten through 12th grade has continued to focus too narrowly on core academic
skills, leaving some critics to argue that the current accountability model is narrowing curriculum,
teaching, and thus learning in schools (Hamilton, Stecher, Marsh, McCombs, & Robyn, 2007).
To support teaching, learning, and assessment based on a more holistic view of CCR, an expanded
framework is required that includes skills in at least four broad domains:
Core academic skills in mathematics, science, and ELA, based on an expanded definition of the
skills and mapped to learning progressions from KCareer.
Cross-cutting capabilities such as critical thinking, collaborative problem solving, as well as
information and technology skills.
Behavioral skills related to success in education and the workforce, such as dependability,
working effectively with others, adapting, and managing stress.
Education and career skills needed to successfully navigate one’s educational and career path,
including self-knowledge of one’s abilities, likes and dislikes, values, etc.; knowledge about majors
and occupations; and a variety of skills related to educational and career exploration, planning,
and decision making.
32
ACT Research Report Broadening the Definition of College and Career Readiness: A Holistic Approach
Core academic skills have been identified in the Common Core State Standards, and other similar
academic standards are already well developed and are being implemented in schools. Standards for
the many other important domains are largely absent, however, from discussions of CCR and from
large-scale assessment. To promote the implementation of this broader view of CCR, a framework
must define the wide array of skills and provide a roadmap of the relevant knowledge and skills
important for readiness across the developmental continuum. Such a framework will allow educators
and students to more clearly understand the precursors of education and work success and will
facilitate the development of a wider range of assessments to track student progress. Specifically,
through clear articulation of each of the domains, and by connecting the knowledge and skills
needed for success to useful and practical assessment, students will receive insights that can drive
action. For example, showing students the ways in which they are off track for readiness and the
skills they need to build to achieve their goals can motivate them to build those skills. To promote
the adoption of such a holistic view of CCR, these insights will need to be linked to supporting
interventions and professional development.
The ultimate goal in developing a more holistic view of CCR is to provide more comprehensive,
personalized, and developmentally appropriate feedback to individuals, offering timely insights in the
form of actionable information to help people achieve education and workplace success.
33
ACT Research Report Broadening the Definition of College and Career Readiness: A Holistic Approach
References
Abele, A. E., & Spurk, D. (2009). The longitudinal impact of self-efficacy and career goals on
objective and subjective career success.Journal of Vocational Behavior,74 , 53–62.
Achieve. (2007). Aligned expectations: A closer look at college admissions and placement tests.
Washington, DC: Achieve. Retrieved October 22, 2011, from http://www.achieve.org/files/
Admissions_and_Placement_FINAL2.pdf
ACT. (2004). Crisis at the core: Preparing all students for college and work. Iowa City, IA: ACT.
ACT. (2006). Ready for college, ready for work: Same or different. Retrieved from http://www.act.
org/research/policymakers/pdf/ReadinessBrief.pdf
ACT. (2007). ACT technical manual. Iowa City, IA: ACT.
ACT. (2008). The forgotten middle: Ensuring that all students are on target for college and career
readiness before high school. Iowa City, IA: Author. Retrieved from http://act.org/research/
policymakers/pdf/ForgottenMiddle.pdf
ACT. (2010). Mind the gaps: How college readiness narrows achievement gaps in college success
(Policy Report). Retrieved from http://www.act.org/research/policymakers/reports/mindthegaps.
html
ACT. (2013a). ACT Profile Report–National. Iowa City, IA: ACT.
ACT. (2013b). ACT National Curriculum Survey
®
2012: Policy Implications on Preparing for Higher
Standards. Iowa City, IA: ACT.
Achter, J. A., Lubinski, D., Benbow, C. P, & Eftekhari-Sanjani, H. (1999). Assessing vocational
preferences among gifted adolescents adds incremental validity to abilities: A discriminant
analysis of educational outcomes over a 10-year interval. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91,
777–786.
Alarcon, G., Eschleman, K. J., & Bowling, N. A. (2009). Relationships between personality variables
and burnout: A meta-analysis.Work & Stress,23, 244–263.
Allen, J. (2013). Updating the ACT College Readiness Benchmarks. Iowa City, IA: ACT.
Allen, J., & Robbins, S. (2010). Effects of interest–major congruence, motivation, and academic
performance on timely degree attainment. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 57(1), 23.
Allen, J., & Sconing, J. (2005). Using ACT assessment scores to set benchmarks for college
readiness. ACT Research Report 2005–3. Retrieved October 21, 2011, from http://www.act.org/
research/researchers/reports/pdf/ACT_RR2005-3.pdf
Allensworth, E. (2005). Graduation and Dropout Trends in Chicago: A look at cohorts of students from
1991 through 2004. Chicago: Consortium on Chicago School Research.
Almlund, M., Duckworth, A., Heckman, J. J., & Kautz, T. (2011). Personality Psychology and
Economics. In E. A. Hanushek, S. Machin & L. Woßmann (Eds.), Handbook of the Economics of
Education. Vol. 4 (pp. 1–181). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
34
ACT Research Report Broadening the Definition of College and Career Readiness: A Holistic Approach
American Diploma Partnership (ADP). (2004). Ready or not: Creating a diploma that counts.
Retrieved from http://www.achieve.org/files/ReadyorNot.pdf
American Management Association (AMA). (2001).
American School Counselor Association. (2004). ASCA National Standards for Students. Alexandria,
Virginia. Author.
Ashton, M. C., Lee, K., Perugini, M., Szarota, P., de Vries, R. E., Di Blas, L., Boies, K., De Raad, B.
(2004). A six factor structure of personality-descriptive adjectives: Solutions from psycholexical
studies in seven languages. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 356–366.
Austin, Z., & Gregory, P. A. M. (2007). Evaluating the accuracy of pharmacy students’ self-
assessment skills. American Journal of Pharmaceutical Education, 71, 1–8.
Autor, D. H., Levy, F., & Murnane, R. J. (2003). The skill content of recent technological change: An
empirical exploration. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 118, 1279–1333.
Balfanz , R., & Letgers, N. (2004). Locating the dropout crisis: Which High Schools Produce the
Nation’s Dropouts? Where Are They Located? Who Attends Them? Center for Social Organization
of Schools, Johns Hopkins University.
Barrick, M. R. & Mount, M. K. (1991). The big five personality dimensions and job performance: A
meta-analysis. Personnel Psychology, 44, 1–26.
Barrick, M. R., Mount, M. K., & Judge, T. A. (2001). Personality and performance at the beginning
of the new millennium: What do we know and where do we go next? International Journal of
Selection and Assessment, 9, 9–29.
Berkner, L., & Chavez, L. (1997). Access to Postsecondary Education for the 1992 High School
Graduates. (NCES 98-105). Washington, DC: US Department of Education, National Center for
Education Statistics.
Berry, C. M., Ones, D. S., & Sackett, P. R. (2007). Interpersonal deviance, organizational deviance,
and their common correlates: A review and meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92,
410–424.
Binet, A., & Simon, T. (1916). The development of intelligence in children (The Binet-Simon Scale).
Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins Co.
Binkley, M., Erstad, O., Herman, J., Raizen, S., Ripley, M., Miller-Ricci, M., & Rumble, M. (2012). Defining
twenty-first century skills. InAssessment and teaching of 21st century skills(pp. 17–66). Springer
Netherlands.
Borman, W. C., & Motowidlo, S. J. (1993). Expanding the criterion domain to include elements of
contextual performance. In N. Schmitt & W. C. Borman (Eds.), Personnel Selection in Organizations
(pp. 71–98). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Borman, W. C., Penner, L. A., Allen, T. D., & Motowidlo, S. J. (2001). Personality predictors of
citizenship performance. International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 9, 52–69.
Bowen, W. G., Chingos, M. M., & McPherson, M. S. (2009). Crossing the finish line: Completing
college at America’s public universities. Princeton University Press.
35
ACT Research Report Broadening the Definition of College and Career Readiness: A Holistic Approach
Brown, S. D., Tramayne, S., Hoxha, D., Telander, K., Fan, X., & Lent, R. W. (2008). Social cognitive
predictors of college students’ academic performance and persistence: A meta-analytic path
analysis.Journal of Vocational Behavior, 72(3), 298–308.
Bureau of Labor Statistics (2013). Overview of projections to 2022. Monthly Labor Review,
December 2013.
Burrus, J., Naemi, B, & Kyllonen, P. C. (2011). Intentional and unintentional faking in education. In M.
Ziegler, C. MacCann, & R. D. Roberts (Eds.), New Perspective on Faking in Personality Assessment
(pp. 282–306). New York: Oxford University Press.
Campbell, J. P. (1990a). Modeling the performance prediction problem in industrial and
organizational psychology. In M. D. Dunnette & L. M. Hough (Eds.), Handbook of Industrial and
Organizational Psychology (pp. 687–732). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press, Inc.
Campbell, J. P. (1990b). An overview of the army selection and classification project (Project A).
Personnel Psychology,43, 231–239.
Campbell, J. P., McCloy, R. A., Oppler, S. H., & Sager, C. E. (1993). A theory of performance: In N.
Schmitt & W. C. Borman (Eds.), Personnel Selection in Organizations (pp. 35–70). San Francisco:
Jossey-Bass.
Camara, W. J. (2005a). Broadening criteria of college success and the impact of cognitive predictors
in admissions testing. In W. J. Camara & E. Kimmel (Eds.), New tools for admissions to higher
education (pp. 81–107). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Camara, W. J. (2005b). Broadening predictors of college success. In W. J. Camara and E. W. Kimmel
(Eds.), Choosing students: Higher education admissions tools for the 21st century (81–105). New
York, NY: Routledge.
Camara, W. (2013). Defining and measuring college and career readiness: A validation framework.
Educational Measurement: Issues and Practice,32, 16–27.
Camara, W.J., Packman, S. and Wiley A. (2013). College, graduate and professional school
admissions testing. In K. Geisinger (Ed.), Handbook of Testing and Assessment in Psychology,
(pp.87–111). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Camara, W. J., & Quenemoen, R. (2012). Defining and measuring CCR and informing the
development of performance level descriptors. Retrieved from http://www.parcconline.org/sites/
parcc/files/PARCCCCRpaperv141-8-12CamaraandQuenemoen.pdf
Campbell, J. P. (1990). Modeling the performance prediction problem in industrial and organizational
psychology. In M. D. Dunnette & L. M. Hough (Eds.), Handbook of Industrial and Organizational
Psychology (687–732). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press, Inc.
Campbell, J. P., McCloy, R. A., Oppler, S. H., & Sager, C. E. (1993). A theory of performance: In
N.Schmitt & W.C. Borman (Eds.), Personnel Selection in Organizations (pp. 35–70).
Carnevale, A. P., & Rose, S. J. (2010).The Undereducated American. Georgetown University, Center
on Education and the Workforce.
Carnevale, A. P., Rose, S. J., & Cheah, B. (2011). The college payoff: Education, occupations, lifetime
earnings. Georgetown University Center on Education and Workforce.
36
ACT Research Report Broadening the Definition of College and Career Readiness: A Holistic Approach
Carnevale, A. P., Smith, N., & Strohl, J. (2010).Help wanted: Projections of job and education
requirements through 2018. Lumina Foundation.
Casillas, A., Robbins, S., Allen, J., Kuo, Y. L., Hanson, M. A., & Schmeiser, C. (2012). Predicting early
academic failure in high school from prior academic achievement, psychosocial characteristics,
and behavior.Journal of Educational Psychology,104, 407–420.
Casner-Lotto, J., & Barrington, L. (2006). Are they really ready to work? Employers’ Perspectives on
the Basic Knowledge and Applied Skills of New Entrants to the 21st US Workforce. Unites States:
The Conference Board, Corporate Voices for Working Families, Partnership for 21st Century
Skills and Society for Human Resource Management.
Clinedinst, M. E., Hurley, S. F., & Hawkins, D. A. (2011). State of college admission. Washington, DC:
National Association for College Admission Counseling.
Chiaburu, D. S., Oh, I., Berry, C. M., Li, N., & Gardner, R. G. (2011). The relationship between Big Five
personality traits and citizenship behaviors: A meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 96,
1140–1166.
Christiansen, N. D., Burns, G. N., & Montgomery, G. E. (2005). Reconsidering forced-choice item
formats for applicant personality assessment. Human Performance, 18, 267–307.
Clinedinst, M. E., & Hawkins, D. A. (2011). State of college admission. Washington, DC: National
Association for College Admission Counseling.
College Board (2012). The 2012 SAT report on CCR. New York, NY: The College Board.
College Board (2013). Using Readistep results and skills insight. New York, NY: The College Board.
Colquitt, J. A., LePine, J. A., & Noe, R. A. (2000). Toward an integrative theory of training motivation:
a meta-analytic path analysis of 20 years of research. Journal of Applied Psychology,85(5),
678707.
Conley, D. (2003). Standards for Success. Understanding University Success. Eugene, OR: Center
for Educational Policy Research, 2003.
Conley, D. T. (2007). Redefining College Readiness. Eugene, OR: Educational Policy Improvement
Center.
Conley, D. T. (2011). Redefining College Readiness, Vol. 5. Eugene, OR: Educational Policy
Improvement Center.
Conley (2012). A complete definition of CCR. Eugene OR: Educational Policy Improvement Center.
Retrieved from https://epiconline.org/Issues/college-career-readiness/definition.dot
Credé, M., & Kuncel, N. R. (2008). Study habits, skills, and attitudes: The third pillar supporting
collegiate academic performance.Perspectives on Psychological Science,3, 425–453.
Cunha, F., & Heckman, J. (2007).The technology of skill formation(No. w12840). National Bureau of
Economic Research.
Deloitte & The Manufacturing Institute. (2011). Boiling point? The skills gap in U.S. manufacturing.
Retrieved from http://www.themanufacturinginstitute.org/~/media/A07730B2A798437D98501
E798C2E13AA.ashx.
37
ACT Research Report Broadening the Definition of College and Career Readiness: A Holistic Approach
Dougherty, C. (2014). Catching Up to College and Career Readiness: The Challenge is Greater for
At-Risk Students. (Policy report). Retrieved from http://www.act.org/research/policymakers/pdf/
CatchingUp-Part3.pdf
Duncan, G. J., Dowsett, C. J., Claessens, A., Mugnuson, K., Huston, A. C., Klebanov, P., Pagani, L. S.,
Feinstein, L., Engel, M., Brooks-Gunn, J., Sexton, H., Duckworth, K., & Japel, C. (2007). School
Readiness and later achievement. Developmental Psychology, 43, 1428–1446.
Dunning, D., Heath, C., & Suls, J. (2004). Flawed self-assessment: Implications for health, education,
and the workplace. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 5, 69–106.
Education Week (2013, May). As graduation rates rise, focus shifts to dropouts. Retrieved from http://
www.edweek.org/ew/articles/2013/06/06/34execsum.h32.html
Education Week (2014, May). US Graduation Rate: Same Results, Different Measures. Retrieved from
http://www.edweek.org/ew/articles/2014/05/07/30gradrate.h33.html
Education Week (2014, June). Motivation Matters: Engaging Student, Creating Learners. Retrieved
from http://www.edweek.org/ew/toc/2014/06/05/index.html?intc=EW-DPCT14-FL1
Ehrlinger, J., Johnson, K., Banner, M., Dunning, D., & Kruger, J. (2008). Why the unskilled are
unaware: Further explorations of (absent) self-insight among the incompetent. Organizational
Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 105, 98–21.
Flay, B. R., & Allred, C. G. (2010). The positive action program: Improving academics, behavior, and
character by teaching comprehensive skills for successful learning and living. In International
research handbook on values education and student wellbeing (pp. 471–501). Springer
Netherlands.
Gallup (2013). America’s Call for Higher Education Redesign: The 2012 Lumina Foundation Study of
the American Public’s Opinion on Higher Education. Retrieved from http://www.luminafoundation.
org/publications/Americas_Call_for_Higher_Education_Redesign.pdf
Greene, J. P., & Winters, M. A. (2005). Public high school graduation and college-readiness rates:
1991–2002. (Education Working Paper No. 8, February 2005). Manhattan Institute for Public
Research. Retrieved from http://www.manhattan-institute.org
Griffith, RL, & Converse, PD (2011). The rules of evidence and the prevalence of applicant faking.
In M. Ziegler, C. MacCann, & R. D. Roberts (Eds.), New perspectives on faking in personality
assessment (pp. 34–52). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Hamilton, L. S., Stecher, B. M., Marsh, J. A., McCombs, J. S., & Robyn, A. (2007). Standards-based
accountability under No Child Left Behind: Experiences of teachers and administrators in three
states. Rand Corporation.
Hanushek, E. A., & Woessmann, L. (2008). The role of cognitive skills in economic development.
Journal of Economic literature, 607–668.
Hart Research Associates. (2010). Raising the bar: Employers’ views on college learning in the wake
of the economic downturn. Washington, DC: Author.
Heckman, J. J., Humphries, J. E., & Kautz, T. (2014). The Myth of Achievement Tests. Chicago, IL: The
University of Chicago Press.
38
ACT Research Report Broadening the Definition of College and Career Readiness: A Holistic Approach
Heggestad, E. D., Morrison, M., Reeve, C. L., & McCloy, R. A. (2006). Forced-choice assessments
of personality for selection: Evaluating issues of normative assessment and faking resistance.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 9–24.
Herman, J. and Linn, R.L. (March, 2013). New assessments, new rigor. Educational leadership, 71(6),
34–37.
Highhouse, S. (2008). Stubborn reliance on intuition and subjectivity in employee selection. Industrial
and Organizational Psychology, 1(3), 333–342.
Holland, J. L. (1997).Making vocational choices: A theory of vocational personalities and work
environments. (3rd ed.). Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
Hunter, J. E. (1980). Test validation for 12,000 jobs: An application of synthetic validity and validity
generalization to the General Aptitude Test Battery (GATB). Washington, DC: US Employment
Service.
Hunter, J. E. (1983). Test validation for 12,000 jobs: An application of job classification and validity
generalization analysis to the General Aptitude Test Battery (GATB) (Test Research Rep. No. 45).
Washington, DC: US Employment Service, US Department of Labor.
Hurtz, G. M., & Donovan, J. J. (2000). Personality and job performance: the Big Five revisited.Journal
of Applied Psychology,85, 869–879.
Information Technology Industry Council, the Partnership for a New American Economy, and the
US Chamber of Commerce (2012). Help wanted: The role of foreign workers in the innovation
economy. Washington, DC.
James, L. R. (1998). Measurement of personality via conditional reasoning. Organizational Research
Methods, 1, 131–163.
John, O. P., & Srivastava, S. (1999). The Big-Five trait taxonomy: History, measurement, and
theoretical perspectives. In L. A. Pervin & O. P. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and
research (Vol. 2, pp. 102–138). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Judge, T. A., & Bono, J. E. (2001). Relationship of core self-evaluations traits—self-esteem,
generalized self-efficacy, locus of control, and emotional stability—with job satisfaction and job
performance: A meta-analysis.Journal of Applied Psychology,86, 80–92.
Judge, T. A., Heller, D., & Mount, M. K. (2002). Five-factor model of personality and job satisfaction: a
meta-analysis.Journal of Applied Psychology,87, 530–541.
Judge, T. A., Higgins, C. A., Thoresen, C. J., & Barrick, M. R. (1999). The big five personality traits,
general mental ability, and career success across the life span.Personnel Psychology,53,
621–652.
Judge, T. A., Klinger, R., Simon, L. S., & Yang, I. W. F. (2008). The contributions of personality to
organizational behavior and psychology: Findings, criticisms, and future research directions. Social
and Personality Psychology Compass, 2, 1982–2000.
Judge, T. A., Rodell, J. B., Klinger, R. L., Simon, L. S., & Crawford, E. R. (2013). Hierarchical
representations of the five-factor model of personality in predicting job performance: Integrating
three organizing frameworks with two theoretical perspectives. Journal of Applied Psychology, 98,
875–925.
39
ACT Research Report Broadening the Definition of College and Career Readiness: A Holistic Approach
King, G., Murray, C. L., Salomon, J. A., & Tandon, A. (2004). Enhancing the validity and cross-cultural
comparability of measurement in survey research. American Political Science Review, 98,
191–207.
Kobrin, J. (2007). Determining SAT benchmarks for college readiness. College Board Research Note
RN-30. New York, NY: The College Board.
Kristof-Brown, A. L., Zimmerman, R. D., & Johnson, E. C. (2005). Consequences of individuals’ fit at
work: A meta-analysis of person-job, person-organization, person-group, and person-supervisor
fit.Personnel Psychology,58, 281–342.
Kruger, J., & Dunning, D. (1999). Unskilled and unaware of it: How difficulties in recognizing
one’s own incompetence lead to inflated self-assessments. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 77, 1121–1134.
Kuncel, N. R., Hezlett, S. A., & Ones, D. S. (2001). A comprehensive meta-analysis of the predictive
validity of the Graduate Record Examinations: Implications for graduate student selection and
performance. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 162–181.
Kyllenon, P. (2008). The Research Behind the ETS
®
Personal Potential Index (PPI). Princeton: NJ,
ETS.
Lievens, F., Buyse, T., & Sackett, P. R. (2005). The operational validity of a video-based situational
judgment test for medical college admissions: Illustrating the importance of matching predictor
and criterion construct domains. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90, 442–452.
Lievens, F., Buyse, T., Sackett, P. R., & Connelly, B. S. (2012). The Effects of Coaching on Situational
Judgment Tests in High-stakes Selection. International Journal of Selection and Assessment,
20(3), 272–282.
Lindqvist, E., & Vestman, R. (2011). The labor market returns to cognitive and noncognitive ability:
Evidence from the Swedish enlistment.American Economic Journal: Applied Economics, 3,
101–28.
Linn, R. L., & Hastings, C. N. (1984). A meta-analysis of the validity of predictors of performance in
law school. Journal of Educational Measurement, 21, 245–259.
Lleras, C. (2008). Do skills and behaviors in high school matter? The contribution of noncognitive
factors in explaining differences in educational attainment and earnings.Social Science Research,
37, 888–902.
Loomis, S. (2011, April). College readiness, career readiness: Same or different? Paper presented at
the meeting of the National Council on Measurement in Education, New Orleans, LA.
Manyika, J., Lund, S., Auguste, B., Mendonca, L., Welsh, T., & Ramaswamy, S. (2011). An economy that
works: Job creation and America’s future. McKinsey Global Institute. Retrieved from http://www.
mckinsey.com/Insights/MGI/Research/Labor_Markets/An_economy_that_works_for_US_job_
creation.
Markle, R., Olivera-Aguilar, M., Jackson, T., Noeth, R., & Robbins, S. (2013). Examining Evidence of
Reliability, Validity, and Fairness for the SuccessNavigator™ Assessment. Princeton: NJ, ETS.
Masten, A. S., & Cicchetti, D. (2010). Developmental cascades. Development and psychopathology,
22(03), 491–495.
40
ACT Research Report Broadening the Definition of College and Career Readiness: A Holistic Approach
Mattern, K. D., Burrus, J., & Shaw, E. J. (2010). When both the skilled and unskilled are unaware:
Consequences for academic performance. Self and Identity, 9, 129–141.
Mattern, K., Kobrin, J., Patterson, B., Shaw, E., & Camara, W. (2009). Validity is in the eye of the
beholder: Conveying SAT research findings to the general public. The concept of validity:
Revisions, new directions, and applications, 213–40.
Mattern, K. D. & Patterson, B. F. (2011a). The Validity of the SAT for Predicting Fourth-Year Grades:
2006 SAT Validity Sample. (College Board Statistical Report 2011-7). New York, NY: The College
Board.
Mattern, K. D. & Patterson, B. F. (2011b). The Relationship between SAT Scores and Retention to the
Fourth Year: 2006 Cohort. (College Board Statistical Report 2011-6). New York, NY: The College
Board.
Mattern, K. D., Shaw, E. J. & Marini, J. (2013). Does college readiness translate to college
completion? College Board Research Note RN 2013-9. New York, NY: The College Board.
McDaniel, M. A., Morgesen, F. P., Finnegan, E. B., Campion, M. A., & Braverman, E. P. (2001). Use of
situational judgment tests to predict job performance: A clarification of the literature. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 86, 730–740.
McHenry, J. J., Hough, L. M., Toquam, J. L., Hanson, M. A., & Ashworth, S. (1990). Project A validity
results: The relationship between predictor and criterion domains.Personnel Psychology,43,
335–354.
Moon, S. H. (2012). Decomposing Racial Skill Gaps in the US. Unpublished manuscript, University of
Chicago, Department of Economics.
Motowildo, S. J. (2003). Job performance. In W. C. Borman, D. R. Ilgen, & R. J. Klimoski (Eds.),
Comprehensive Handbook of Psychology Vol. 12. Industrial and Organizational Psychology
(pp.39–53). New York, NY: Wiley.
Mumford, M. D., & Owens, W. A. (1987). Methodology review: Principles, procedures, and findings in
the application of background data measures. Applied Psychological Measurement, 11, 1–31.
National Career Development Association (2009). National Career Development Guidelines (NCDG)
Framework. Retrieved from http://www.ncda.org/aws/NCDA/asset_manager/get_file/3384/
ncdguidelines2009.pdf
Neild, R. C., & Balfanz, R. (2006). Unfulfilled promise: The dimensions and characteristics of
Philadelphia’s dropout crisis, 2000–2005. Retrieved from http://www.csos.jhu.edu/new/Neild_
Balfanz_06.pdf.
Newport, F., & Busteed, B. (2013). Americans Still See College Education as Very Important: Seven in
10 say college education is very important, up significantly from 1978. Retrieved from http://www.
gallup.com/poll/166490/americans-college-education-important.aspx
Nye, C. D., Su, R., Rounds, J., & Drasgow, F. (2012). Vocational interests and performance a
quantitative summary of over 60 years of research.Perspectives on Psychological Science,7,
384–403.
O’Connor, M. C., & Paunonen, S. V. (2007). Big Five personality predictors of post-secondary
academic performance.Personality and Individual differences, 43, 971–990.
41
ACT Research Report Broadening the Definition of College and Career Readiness: A Holistic Approach
Ones, D. S., Viswesvaran, C., & Schmidt, F. L. (1993). Comprehensive meta-analysis of integrity test
validities: Findings and implications for personnel selection and theories of job performance.
Journal of Applied Psychology, 78, 679–703.
Organ, D. W., & Ryan, K. (1995). A meta-analytic review of attitudinal and dispositional predictors of
organizational citizenship behavior. Personnel Psychology, 48, 775–802.
Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development (OECD) (2013a). OECD Skills
Outlook 2013: First Results from the Survey of Adult Skills, OECD Publishing. http://dx.doi.
org/10.1787/9789264204256-en
Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development (OECD) (2013b). Time for the US to
Reskill?: What the Survey of Adult Skills Says, OECD Skills Studies, OECD Publishing. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1787/9789264204904-en
Oswald, F. L., Schmitt, N., Kim, B. H., Ramsay, L. J., & Gillespie, M. A. (2004). Developing a biodata
measure and situational judgment inventory as predictors of college student performance. Journal
of Applied Psychology, 89(2), 187.
Parsad, B., Lewis, L., & Greene, B. (2003).Remedial education at degree-granting postsecondary
institutions in Fall 2000 (NCES 2004-010). Washington, DC: National Center for Education
Statistics.
Partnership for 21st Century Skills (2014). Framework for 21st Century Learning. Retrieved from
http://www.p21.org/storage/documents/1.__p21_framework_2-pager.pdf
Pascarella, E. & Terenzini, P. (2005). How college affects students: Findings and insights from twenty
years of research. Volume 2: A third decade of research. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
Poropat, A. E. (2009). A meta-analysis of the five-factor model of personality and academic
performance. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 322–338.
Prelec, D. (2004). A Bayesian truth serum for subjective data. Science, 306, 462–466.
Presley, J. B., & Gong, Y. (2005). The Demographics and Academics of College Readiness in Illinois,
IERC 2005-3, (Edwardsville, IL: Illinois Education Research Council, 2005). Retrieved from www.
schooldata4all.org.
Proctor, T., Wyatt, J., & Wiley, A. (2010). PSAT/NMSQT Indicators of college readiness (College
Board Research Report 2010-4). New York, NY: The College Board.
Radunzel, J., & Noble, J. (2012b). Predicting long-term college success through degree completion
using ACT composite score, ACT benchmarks, and high school grade point average (Research
Report 2012-5). Retrieved from http://media.act.org/documents/ACT_RR2012-5.pdf
Richardson, M., Abraham, C., & Bond, R. (2012). Psychological correlates of university students’
academic performance: a systematic review and meta-analysis.Psychological Bulletin,138,
353–387.
Robbins, S. B., Lauver, K., Le, H., Davis, D., Langley, R., & Carlstrom, A. (2004). Do psychosocial and
study skill factors predict college outcomes? A meta-analysis. Psychological bulletin, 130(2), 261.
Roberts, B. W., Kuncel, N. R., Shiner, R., Caspi, A., & Goldberg, L. R. (2007). The power of personality:
The comparative validity of personality traits, socioeconomic status, and cognitive ability for
predicting important life outcomes.Perspectives on Psychological Science,2, 313–345.
42
ACT Research Report Broadening the Definition of College and Career Readiness: A Holistic Approach
Roseth, C. J., Johnson, D. W., & Johnson, R. T. (2008). Promoting early adolescents’ achievement
and peer relationships: The effects of cooperative, competitive, and individualistic goal
structures.Psychological bulletin,134(2), 223.
Sackett, P. (2011). Faking in personality assessments: Where do we stand? In M. Ziegler,
C.MacCann, & R. D. Roberts (Eds.), New perspectives on faking in personality assessment
(pp.330–344). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Salgado, J. F. (2002). The big five personality dimensions and counterproductive behaviors.
International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 10, 117–125.
Salgado, J. F. (2003). Predicting job performance using FFM and non-FFM personality measures.
Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 76, 323–346.
Schmidt, F. L., & Hunter, J. E. (1998). The validity and utility of selection methods in personnel
psychology: Practical and theoretical implications of 85 years of research findings. Psychological
Bulletin, 124, 262–274.
Schmidt, F. L., & Hunter, J. (2004). General mental ability in the world of work: occupational
attainment and job performance.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,86, 162–173.
Schmitt, N., Keeney, J., Oswald, F. L., Pleskac, T. J., Billington, A. Q., Sinha, R., & Zorzie, M. (2009).
Prediction of 4-year college student performance using cognitive and noncognitive predictors
and the impact on demographic status of admitted students.Journal of Applied Psychology,94,
14791497.
Schmitt, N., & Kuncel, C. (2002). The effects of required elaboration of answers to biodata questions.
Personnel Psychology, 55, 569–587.
Shultz, M. M., & Zedeck, S. (2011). Predicting lawyer effectiveness: Broadening the basis for law
school admission decisions. Law & Social Inquiry, 36(3), 620–661.
Smarter Balanced Assessment Consortia (SBAC). (2012). How will Smarter Balanced validate its
CCR benchmarks? Retrieved from http://www.smarterbalanced.org/resources-events/faqs/
Society for Human Resource Management (SHRM). (2012). Most Employers Don’t Use Personality
Tests. Retrieved from http://www.shrm.org/hrdisciplines/staffingmanagement/articles/pages/
mostemployersdontusepersonalitytests.aspx
Stark, S., Chernyshenko, O. S., & Drasgow, F. (2005). An IRT approach to constructing and scoring
pairwise preference items involving stimuli on different dimensions: An application to the problem
of faking in personality assessment. Applied Psychological Measurement, 29, 184–201.
Stark, S., Chernyshenko, O. S., & Drasgow, F. (2010). Tailored Adaptive Personality Assessment
System (TAPAS-95s). Expanded enlistment eligibility metrics (EEEM): Recommendations on a
non-cognitive screen for new soldier selection.
Trapmann, S., Hell, B., Hirn, J. O. W., & Schuler, H. (2007). Meta-analysis of the relationship
between the Big Five and academic success at university. Zeitschrift für Psychologie/Journal of
Psychology,215, 132–151.
US Department of Education, Institute of Education Sciences, National Center for Education
Statistics, National Assessment of Educational Progress (NAEP), 2013 Mathematics and Reading
Assessments.
43
ACT Research Report Broadening the Definition of College and Career Readiness: A Holistic Approach
US Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics. (2013a). Digest of Education
Statistics, 2012 (NCES 2014-015). Washington, DC: National Center for Education Statistics.
US Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics. (2013b). First Year
Undergraduate Remedial Coursetaking: 1999–2000, 2003–2004, 2007–2008 (NCES 2013-
013). Washington, DC: National Center for Education Statistics.
Valentine, J. C., DuBois, D. L., & Cooper, H. (2004). The relation between self-beliefs and academic
achievement: A meta-analytic review. Educational Psychology, 39, 111–133.
Van Iddekinge, C. H., Putka, D. J., & Campbell, J. P. (2011). Reconsidering vocational interests for
personnel selection: The validity of an interest-based selection test in relation to job knowledge,
job performance, and continuance intentions.Journal of Applied Psychology,96(1), 13.
Van Iddekinge, C. H., Roth, P. L., Raymark, P. H., & Odle-Dusseau (2012). The criterion-related validity
of integrity tests: An updated meta-analysis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 97, 499–530.
Weinstein, C. E., Schulte, A., & Palmer, D. R. (1987). The Learning and Study Strategies Inventory.
Clearwater, FL: H & H Publishing.
Wiley, A., Wyatt, J., & Camara, W. J. (2010). The development of a multidimensional college
readiness index. College Board Research Report 2010–3. Retrieved from https://research.
collegeboard.org/sites/default/files/publications/2012/7/researchreport-2010-3-development-
multidimensional-college-readiness-index.pdf
Wyatt, J., Kobrin, J., Wiley, A., Camara, W. J., & Proestler, N. (2011). SAT benchmarks: Development
of a college readiness benchmark and its relationship to secondary and post secondary school
performance. College Board Research Report 2011–5. Retrieved from http://research.
collegeboard.org/sites/default/files/publications/2012/7/researchreport-2011-5-sat-college-
readiness-benchmark-secondary-performance.pdf
Wyatt, J., Wiley, A., Camara, W. J., & Proestler, N. (2011). The development of an index of academic
rigor for college readiness. Retrieved from http://research.collegeboard.org/sites/default/files/
publications/2012/7/researchreport-2011-11-development-index-academic-rigor-college-
success.pdf
Zimmerman, R. D. (2008). Understanding the impact of personality traits on individuals’ turnover
decisions: A meta-analytic path model. Personnel Psychology,61, 309–348.
*050205140* Rev 1